
The neutrality of this article is disputed.(November 2016) |
Gestalt psychology, gestaltism, or configurationism is a school of psychology and a theory of perception that emphasises the processing of entire patterns and configurations, and not merely individual components. It emerged in the early twentieth century in Austria and Germany as a rejection of basic principles of Wilhelm Wundt's and Edward Titchener's elementalist and structuralist psychology.
Gestalt psychology is often associated with the adage, "The whole is greater than the sum of its parts". In Gestalt theory, information is perceived as wholes rather than disparate parts which are then processed summatively. As used in Gestalt psychology, the German word Gestalt (/ɡəˈʃtælt, -ˈʃtɑːlt/ gə-SHTA(H)LT,German: [ɡəˈʃtalt] ; meaning "form") is interpreted as "pattern" or "configuration".
It differs from Gestalt therapy, which is only peripherally linked to Gestalt psychology.
Origin and history
Max Wertheimer, Kurt Koffka, and Wolfgang Köhler founded Gestalt psychology in the early 20th century.: 113–116 The dominant view in psychology at the time was structuralism, exemplified by the work of Hermann von Helmholtz, Wilhelm Wundt, and Edward B. Titchener.: 3 Structuralism was rooted firmly in British empiricism: 3 and was based on three closely interrelated theories:
- "atomism," also known as "elementalism,": 3 the view that all knowledge, even complex abstract ideas, is built from simple, elementary constituents
- "sensationalism," the view that the simplest constituents—the atoms of thought—are elementary sense impressions
- "associationism," the view that more complex ideas arise from the association of simpler ideas.: 3
Together, these three theories give rise to the view that the mind constructs all perceptions and abstract thoughts strictly from lower-level sensations, which are related solely by being associated closely in space and time. The Gestaltists took issue with the widespread atomistic view that the aim of psychology should be to break consciousness down into putative basic elements.
In contrast, the Gestalt psychologists believed that breaking psychological phenomena down into smaller parts would not lead to understanding psychology.: 13 Instead, they viewed psychological phenomena as organized, structured wholes.: 13 They argued that the psychological "whole" has priority and that the "parts" are defined by the structure of the whole, rather than the other way round. Gestalt theories of perception are based on human nature being inclined to understand objects as an entire structure rather than the sum of its parts.
Wertheimer had been a student of Austrian philosopher, Christian von Ehrenfels, a member of the School of Brentano. Von Ehrenfels introduced the concept of Gestalt to philosophy and psychology in 1890, before the advent of Gestalt psychology as such. Von Ehrenfels observed that a perceptual experience, such as perceiving a melody or a shape, is more than the sum of its sensory components. He claimed that, in addition to the sensory elements of the perception, there is something additional that is an element in its own right, despite in some sense being derived from the organization of the component sensory elements. He called it Gestalt-qualität or "form-quality." It is this Gestalt-qualität that, according to von Ehrenfels, allows a tune to be transposed to a new key, using completely different notes, while still retaining its identity. The idea of a Gestalt-qualität has roots in theories by David Hume, Johann Wolfgang von Goethe, Immanuel Kant, David Hartley, and Ernst Mach. Both von Ehrenfels and Edmund Husserl seem to have been inspired by Mach's work Beiträge zur Analyse der Empfindungen (Contributions to the Analysis of Sensations, 1886), in formulating their very similar concepts of gestalt and figural moment, respectively.
By 1914, the first published references to Gestalt theory could be found in a footnote of Gabriele von Wartensleben's application of Gestalt theory to personality. She was a student at Frankfurt Academy for Social Sciences, who interacted deeply with Wertheimer and Köhler.
Through a series of experiments, Wertheimer discovered that a person observing a pair of alternating bars of light can, under the right conditions, experience the illusion of movement between one location and the other. He noted that this was a perception of motion absent any moving object. That is, it was pure phenomenal motion. He dubbed it phi ("phenomenal") motion. Wertheimer's publication of these results in 1912 marks the beginning of Gestalt psychology. In comparison to von Ehrenfels and others who had used the term "gestalt" earlier in various ways, Wertheimer's unique contribution was to insist that the "gestalt" is perceptually primary. The gestalt defines the parts from which it is composed, rather than being a secondary quality that emerges from those parts. Wertheimer took the more radical position that one hears the melody first and only then may perceptually divide it up into notes. Similarly, in vision, one sees the form of the circle first, with its apprehension not mediated by a process of part-summation. Only after this primary apprehension might one notice that it is made up of lines or dots or stars.
The two men who served as Wertheimer's subjects in the phi experiments were Köhler and Koffka. Köhler was an expert in physical acoustics, having studied under physicist Max Planck, but had taken his degree in psychology under Carl Stumpf. Koffka was also a student of Stumpf's, having studied movement phenomena and psychological aspects of rhythm. In 1917, Köhler published the results of four years of research on learning in chimpanzees. Köhler showed, contrary to the claims of most other learning theorists, that animals can learn by "sudden insight" into the "structure" of a problem, over and above the associative and incremental manner of learning that Ivan Pavlov and Edward Lee Thorndike had demonstrated with dogs and cats, respectively.
In 1921, Koffka published a Gestalt-oriented text on developmental psychology, Growth of the Mind. With the help of American psychologist Robert Ogden, Koffka introduced the Gestalt point of view to an American audience in 1922 by way of a paper in Psychological Bulletin. It contains criticisms of then-current explanations of a number of problems of perception, and the alternatives offered by the Gestalt school. Koffka moved to the United States in 1924, eventually settling at Smith College in 1927. In 1935, Koffka published his Principles of Gestalt Psychology. This textbook laid out the Gestalt vision of the scientific enterprise as a whole. Science, he said, is not the simple accumulation of facts. What makes research scientific is the incorporation of facts into a theoretical structure. The goal of the Gestaltists was to integrate the facts of inanimate nature, life, and mind into a single scientific structure. This meant that science would have to accommodate not only what Koffka called the quantitative facts of physical science but the facts of two other "scientific categories": questions of order and questions of Sinn, a German word which has been variously translated as significance, value, and meaning. Without incorporating the meaning of experience and behavior, Koffka believed that science would doom itself to trivialities in its investigation of human beings.
Having survived the Nazis up to the mid-1930s, all the core members of the Gestalt movement were forced out of Germany to the United States by 1935. Köhler published another book, Dynamics in Psychology, in 1940 but thereafter the Gestalt movement suffered a series of setbacks. Koffka died in 1941 and Wertheimer in 1943. Wertheimer's long-awaited book on mathematical problem-solving, Productive Thinking, was published posthumously in 1945, but Köhler was left to guide the movement without his two long-time colleagues.
Gestalt therapy
Gestalt psychology differs from Gestalt therapy, which is only peripherally linked to Gestalt psychology. The founders of Gestalt therapy, Fritz and Laura Perls, had worked with Kurt Goldstein, a neurologist who had applied principles of Gestalt psychology to the functioning of the organism. Laura Perls had been a Gestalt psychologist before she became a psychoanalyst and before she began developing Gestalt therapy together with Fritz Perls. The extent to which Gestalt psychology influenced Gestalt therapy is disputed. On one hand, Laura Perls preferred not to use the term "Gestalt" to name the emerging new therapy, because she thought that the Gestalt psychologists would object to it; on the other hand, Fritz and Laura Perls clearly adopted some of Goldstein's work.
Mary Henle noted in her presidential address to Division 24 at the meeting of the American Psychological Association: "What Perls has done has been to take a few terms from Gestalt psychology, stretch their meaning beyond recognition, mix them with notions—often unclear and often incompatible—from the depth psychologies, existentialism, and common sense, and he has called the whole mixture gestalt therapy. His work has no substantive relation to scientific Gestalt psychology. To use his own language, Fritz Perls has done 'his thing'; whatever it is, it is not Gestalt psychology."
One form of psychotherapy that, unlike Gestalt therapy, is actually consistently based on Gestalt psychology is Gestalt theoretical psychotherapy.
Theoretical framework and methodology
The Gestalt psychologists practiced a set of theoretical and methodological principles that attempted to redefine the approach to psychological research. This is in contrast to investigations developed at the beginning of the 20th century, based on traditional scientific methodology, which divided the object of study into a set of elements that could be analyzed separately with the objective of reducing the complexity of this object.
The principle of totality asserts that conscious experience must be considered globally by taking into account all the physical and mental aspects of the individual simultaneously, because the nature of the mind demands that each component be considered as part of a system of dynamic relationships. Thus, holism as fundamental aspect of Gestalt psychology. Moreover, the perception of the nature of a part depends upon the whole in which it is embedded. The maxim that the whole is more than the sum of its parts is not a precise description of the Gestaltist view. Rather, as Koffka writes, "The whole is something else than the sum of its parts, because summing is a meaningless procedure, whereas the whole-part relationship is meaningful."
The principle of psychophysical isomorphism hypothesizes that there is a correlation between conscious experience and cerebral activity.
Based on the principles, phenomenon experimental analysis was derived, which asserts that any psychological research should take phenomena as a starting point and not be solely focused on sensory qualities. A related principle is that of the biotic experiment, which establishes the need to conduct real experiments that sharply contrasted with and opposed classic laboratory experiments. This signified experimenting in natural situations, developed in real conditions, in which it would be possible to reproduce, with higher fidelity, what would be habitual for a subject.
Principles
The Gestaltists were the first to document and demonstrate empirically many facts about perception—including facts about the perception of movement, the perception of contour, perceptual constancy, and perceptual illusions. Wertheimer's discovery of the phi phenomenon is one example of such a contribution.
Properties
The key principles of gestalt systems are emergence, reification, multistability and invariance. These principles are not necessarily separable modules to model individually, but they could be different aspects of a single unified dynamic mechanism.
Reification

Reification is the constructive or generative aspect of perception, by which the experienced object of perception contains more explicit spatial information than the sensory stimulus on which it is based. For instance, a triangle is perceived in picture A, though no triangle is there. In pictures B and D the eye recognizes disparate shapes as "belonging" to a single shape, in C a complete three-dimensional shape is seen, where in actuality no such thing is drawn.
Reification can be explained by progress in the study of illusory contours, which are treated by the visual system as "real" contours.
Multistability

Multistability (or multistable perception) is the tendency of ambiguous perceptual experiences to pop back and forth between two or more alternative interpretations. This is seen, for example, in the Necker cube and Rubin's Figure/Vase illusion. Other examples include the three-legged blivet, artist M. C. Escher's artwork, and the appearance of flashing marquee lights moving first one direction and then suddenly the other.
Invariance

Invariance is the property of perception whereby simple geometrical objects are recognized independent of rotation, translation, and scale, as well as several other variations such as elastic deformations, different lighting, and different component features. For example, the objects in A in the figure are all immediately recognized as the same basic shape, which is immediately distinguishable from the forms in B. They are even recognized despite perspective and elastic deformations as in C, and when depicted using different graphic elements as in D. Computational theories of vision, such as those by David Marr, have provided alternate explanations of how perceived objects are classified.
Perceptual organisation forms
Perceptual grouping
Like figure-ground organization, perceptual grouping (sometimes called perceptual segregation) is a form of perceptual organization. Perceptual grouping is the process that determines how organisms perceive some parts of their perceptual fields as being more related than others, using such information for object detection.
The Gestaltists were the first psychologists to systematically study perceptual grouping. According to Gestalt psychologists, the fundamental principle of perceptual grouping is the law of Prägnanz, also known as the law of good Gestalt. Prägnanz is a German word that directly translates to "pithiness" and implies salience, conciseness, and orderliness. The law of Prägnanz says that people tend to experience things as regular, orderly, symmetrical, and simple.
Gestalt psychologists attempted to discover refinements of the law of Prägnanz, which involved writing down laws that predict the interpretation of sensation. Wertheimer defined a few principles that explain the ways humans perceive objects based on similarity, proximity, and continuity.
Law of proximity

The law of proximity states that when an individual perceives an assortment of objects, they perceive objects that are close to each other as forming a group. For example, in the figure illustrating the law of proximity, there are 72 circles, but we perceive the collection of circles in groups. Specifically, we perceive that there is a group of 36 circles on the left side of the image and three groups of 12 circles on the right side of the image. This law is often used in advertising logos to emphasize which aspects of events are associated.
Law of similarity

The law of similarity states that elements within an assortment of objects are perceptually grouped together if they are similar to each other. This similarity can occur in the form of shape, colour, shading or other qualities. For example, the figure illustrating the law of similarity portrays 36 circles all equal distance apart from one another forming a square. In this depiction, 18 of the circles are shaded dark, and 18 of the circles are shaded light. We perceive the dark circles as grouped together and the light circles as grouped together, forming six horizontal lines within the square of circles. This perception of lines is due to the law of similarity.
Law of closure

Gestalt psychologists believed that humans tend to perceive objects as complete rather than focusing on the gaps that the object might contain. For example, a circle has good Gestalt in terms of completeness. However, we will also perceive an incomplete circle as a complete circle. That tendency to complete shapes and figures is called closure. The law of closure states that individuals perceive objects such as shapes, letters, pictures, etc., as being whole when they are not complete. Specifically, when parts of a whole picture are missing, our perception fills in the visual gap. Research shows that the reason the mind completes a regular figure that is not perceived through sensation is to increase the regularity of surrounding stimuli. For example, the figure that depicts the law of closure portrays what we perceive as a circle on the left side of the image and a rectangle on the right side of the image. However, gaps are present in the shapes. If the law of closure did not exist, the image would depict an assortment of different lines with different lengths, rotations, and curvatures—but with the law of closure, we perceptually combine the lines into whole shapes.
Law of symmetry

The law of symmetry states that the mind perceives objects as being symmetrical and forming around a center point. It is perceptually pleasing to divide objects into an even number of symmetrical parts. Therefore, when two symmetrical elements are unconnected the mind perceptually connects them to form a coherent shape. Similarities between symmetrical objects increase the likelihood that objects are grouped to form a combined symmetrical object. For example, the figure depicting the law of symmetry shows a configuration of square and curled brackets. When the image is perceived, we tend to observe three pairs of symmetrical brackets rather than six individual brackets.
Law of common fate
The law of common fate states that objects are perceived as lines that move along the smoothest path. Experiments using the visual sensory modality found that the movement of elements of an object produces paths that individuals perceive that the objects are on. We perceive elements of objects to have trends of motion, which indicate the path that the object is on. The law of continuity implies the grouping together of objects that have the same trend of motion and are therefore on the same path. For example, if there is an array of dots and half the dots are moving upward while the other half are moving downward, we would perceive the upward moving dots and the downward moving dots as two distinct units.
Law of continuity

The law of continuity (also known as the law of good continuation) states that elements of objects tend to be grouped together, and therefore integrated into perceptual wholes if they are aligned within an object. In cases where there is an intersection between objects, individuals tend to perceive the two objects as two single uninterrupted entities. Stimuli remain distinct even with overlap. We are less likely to group elements with sharp abrupt directional changes as being one object. For example, the figure depicting the law of continuity shows a configuration of two crossed keys. When the image is perceived, we tend to perceive the key in the background as a single uninterrupted key instead of two separate halves of a key.
Law of past experience
The law of past experience implies that under some circumstances visual stimuli are categorized according to past experience. If objects tend to be observed within close proximity, or small temporal intervals, the objects are more likely to be perceived together. For example, the English language contains 26 letters that are grouped to form words using a set of rules. If an individual reads an English word they have never seen, they use the law of past experience to interpret the letters "L" and "I" as two letters beside each other, rather than using the law of closure to combine the letters and interpret the object as an uppercase U.
Music
An example of the Gestalt movement in effect, as it is both a process and result, is a music sequence. People are able to recognise a sequence of perhaps six or seven notes, despite them being transposed into a different tuning or key. An early theory of gestalt grouping principles in music was composer-theorist James Tenney's Meta+Hodos (1961).Auditory Scene Analysis as developed by Albert Bregman further extends a gestalt approach to the analysis of sound perception.
Figure-ground organization
Figure-ground organization is a form of perceptual organization, which interprets perceptual elements in terms of their shapes and relative locations in the layout of surfaces in the 3-D world. Figure-ground organization structures the perceptual field into a figure (standing out at the front of the perceptual field) and a background (receding behind the figure). Pioneering work on figure-ground organization was carried out by the Danish psychologist Edgar Rubin. The Gestalt psychologists demonstrated that people tend to perceive as figures those parts of our perceptual fields that are convex, symmetric, small, and enclosed.
Problem solving and insight
Gestalt psychology contributed to the scientific study of problem solving. In fact, the early experimental work of the Gestaltists in Germany marks the beginning of the scientific study of problem solving. Later this experimental work continued through the 1960s and early 1970s with research conducted on relatively simple laboratory tasks of problem solving.
Max Wertheimer distinguished two kinds of thinking: productive thinking and reproductive thinking.: 456 : 361 Productive thinking is solving a problem based on insight—a quick, creative, unplanned response to situations and environmental interaction. Reproductive thinking is solving a problem deliberately based on previous experience and knowledge. Reproductive thinking proceeds algorithmically—a problem solver reproduces a series of steps from memory, knowing that they will lead to a solution—or by trial and error.: 361
Karl Duncker, another Gestalt psychologist who studied problem solving,: 370 coined the term functional fixedness for describing the difficulties in both visual perception and problem solving that arise from the fact that one element of a whole situation already has a (fixed) function that has to be changed in order to perceive something or find the solution to a problem.
Legacy
Gestalt psychology struggled to precisely define terms like Prägnanz, to make specific behavioural predictions, and to articulate testable models of underlying neural mechanisms. It was criticized as being merely descriptive. These shortcomings led, by the mid-20th century, to growing dissatisfaction with Gestaltism and a subsequent decline in its impact on psychology. Despite this decline, Gestalt psychology has formed the basis of much further research into the perception of patterns and objects and of research into behaviour, thinking, problem solving and psychopathology.
Support from cybernetics and neurology
In the 1940s and 1950s, laboratory research in neurology and what became known as cybernetics on the mechanism of frogs' eyes indicate that perception of 'gestalts' (in particular gestalts in motion) is perhaps more primitive and fundamental than 'seeing' as such:
- A frog hunts on land by vision... He has no fovea, or region of greatest acuity in vision, upon which he must centre a part of the image... The frog does not seem to see or, at any rate, is not concerned with the detail of stationary parts of the world around him. He will starve to death surrounded by food if it is not moving. His choice of food is determined only by size and movement. He will leap to capture any object the size of an insect or worm, providing it moves like one. He can be fooled easily not only by a piece of dangled meat but by any moving small object... He does remember a moving thing provided it stays within his field of vision and he is not distracted.
- The lowest-level concepts related to visual perception for a human being probably differ little from the concepts of a frog. In any case, the structure of the retina in mammals and in human beings is the same as in amphibians. The phenomenon of distortion of perception of an image stabilised on the retina gives some idea of the concepts of the subsequent levels of the hierarchy. This is a very interesting phenomenon. When a person looks at an immobile object, "fixes" it with his eyes, the eyeballs do not remain absolutely immobile; they make small involuntary movements. As a result, the image of the object on the retina is constantly in motion, slowly drifting and jumping back to the point of maximum sensitivity. The image "marks time" in the vicinity of this point.
Use in contemporary social psychology
The halo effect can be explained through the application of Gestalt theories to social information processing. The constructive theories of social cognition are applied to the expectations of individuals. They have been perceived in this manner and the person judging the individual is continuing to view them in this positive manner. Gestalt's theories of perception enforces that individual's tendency to perceive actions and characteristics as a whole rather than isolated parts, therefore humans are inclined to build a coherent and consistent impression of objects and behaviors in order to achieve an acceptable shape and form. The halo effect is what forms patterns for individuals, the halo effect being classified as a cognitive bias which occurs during impression formation. The halo effect can also be altered by physical characteristics, social status and many other characteristics. As well, the halo effect can have real repercussions on the individual's perception of reality, either negatively or positively, meaning to construct negative or positive images about other individuals or situations, something that could lead to self-fulfilling prophesies, stereotyping, or even discrimination.
Contemporary cognitive and perceptual psychology
Some of the central criticisms of Gestaltism are based on the preference Gestaltists are deemed to have for theory over data, and a lack of quantitative research supporting Gestalt ideas. This is not necessarily a fair criticism as highlighted by a recent collection of quantitative research on Gestalt perception. Researchers continue to test hypotheses about the mechanisms underlying Gestalt principles such as the principle of similarity.
Other important criticisms concern the lack of definition and support for the many physiological assumptions made by gestaltists and lack of theoretical coherence in modern Gestalt psychology.
In some scholarly communities, such as cognitive psychology and computational neuroscience, gestalt theories of perception are criticized for being descriptive rather than explanatory in nature. For this reason, they are viewed by some as redundant or uninformative. For example, a textbook on visual perception states that, "The physiological theory of the gestaltists has fallen by the wayside, leaving us with a set of descriptive principles, but without a model of perceptual processing. Indeed, some of their 'laws' of perceptual organisation today sound vague and inadequate. What is meant by a 'good' or 'simple' shape, for example?"
One historian of psychology, David J. Murray, has argued that Gestalt psychologists first discovered many principles later championed by cognitive psychology, including schemas and prototypes. Another psychologist has argued that the Gestalt psychologists made a lasting contribution by showing how the study of illusions can help scientists understand essential aspects of how the visual system normally functions, not merely how it breaks down.: 16
Use in design

The gestalt laws are used in several visual design fields, such as user interface design and cartography. The laws of similarity and proximity can, for example, be used as guides for placing radio buttons. They may also be used in designing computers and software for more intuitive human use. Examples include the design and layout of a desktop's shortcuts in rows and columns.
In map design, principles of Prägnanz or grouping are crucial for implying a conceptual order to the portrayed geographic features, thus facilitating the intended use of the map. The Law of Similarity is employed by selecting similar map symbols for similar kinds of features or features with similar properties; the Law of Proximity is crucial to identifying geographic patterns and regions; and the Laws of Closure and Continuity allow users to recognize features that may be obscured by other features (such as when a road goes over a river).
See also
- Augusto Garau
- Amodal perception
- Cognitive grammar
- Egregore
- Gestaltzerfall
- Graz School
- Hans Wallach
- Hermann Friedmann
- James J. Gibson
- James Tenney
- Laws of association
- Mereology
- Optical illusion
- Pál Schiller Harkai
- Pattern recognition (machine learning)
- Pattern recognition (psychology)
- Phenomenology
- Principles of grouping
- Rudolf Arnheim
- Solomon Asch
- Structural information theory
- Topological data analysis
- Wolfgang Metzger
- Vera Felicidade de Almeida Campos
Notes
- For more on the history of Gestalt psychology, see Ash, M. G. (1995). Gestalt psychology in German culture, 1890–1967: Holism and the quest for objectivity. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press..
- For example, Duncker, Karl (1935). Zur Psychologie des produktiven Denkens [The psychology of productive thinking] (in German). Berlin: Julius Springer.
- For example Duncker's "X-ray" problem; Ewert & Lambert's "disk" problem in 1932, later known as Tower of Hanoi.
References
- Mather, George (2006). Foundations of Perception. Psychology Press. p. 32. ISBN 978-0-86377-834-6.
- "Gestalt psychology". Encyclopædia Britannica. Retrieved 2 January 2023.
- Baker, David B., ed. (13 January 2012). The Oxford Handbook of the History of Psychology: Global Perspectives. Oxford University Press, USA. p. 576. ISBN 978-0-19-536655-6.
- Wells, John (3 April 2008). Longman Pronunciation Dictionary (3rd ed.). Pearson Longman. ISBN 978-1-4058-8118-0.
- "Definition of gestalt | Dictionary.com". www.dictionary.com. Retrieved 2 January 2023.
- "Gestalt". The Columbia Encyclopedia. Columbia University Press. 2018. ISBN 9781786848468. Archived from the original on 22 July 2019. Retrieved 1 August 2019.
- "Gestalt psychology". Britannica Concise Encyclopedia. Encyclopaedia Britannica, Inc. 1 May 2008. p. 756. ISBN 9781593394929. Archived from the original on 15 April 2021. Retrieved 2 October 2020.
- Sternberg, Robert J.; Sternberg, Karin (2012). Cognitive Psychology (6th ed.). Belmont, CA: Cengage Learning. ISBN 978-1-133-31391-5.
- Wagemans, Johan; Feldman, Jacob; Gepshtein, Sergei; Kimchi, Ruth; Pomerantz, James R.; van der Helm, Peter A.; van Leeuwen, Cees (2012). "A century of Gestalt psychology in visual perception: II. Conceptual and theoretical foundations". Psychological Bulletin. 138 (6): 1218–1252. doi:10.1037/a0029334. ISSN 1939-1455. PMC 3728284. PMID 22845750.
- Kolers, Paul A. (1972). Aspects of Motion Perception: International Series of Monographs in Experimental Psychology. New York: Pergamon. ISBN 978-1-4831-7113-5.
- Hamlyn, D. W. (1957). The Psychology of Perception: A Philosophical Examination of Gestalt Theory and Derivative Theories of Perception (eBook ed.). London: Routledge. pp. 88–89. ISBN 978-1-315-47329-1. Archived from the original on 21 October 2020. Retrieved 19 October 2019.
- Verstegen, Ian (2010). "Gestalt Psychology". The Corsini Encyclopedia of Psychology. pp. 1–3. doi:10.1002/9780470479216.corpsy0386. ISBN 9780470479216.
- Pohl, Rüdiger F. (22 July 2016). Cognitive Illusions: Intriguing Phenomena in Judgement, Thinking and Memory. Psychology Press. ISBN 9781317448280. Archived from the original on 15 April 2021. Retrieved 2 October 2020.
- Smith, Barry (1988). "Gestalt Theory: An Essay in Philosophy". In Smith, Barry (ed.). Foundations of Gestalt Theory. Vienna: Philosophia Verlag. pp. 11–81. Archived from the original (PDF) on 22 February 2012. Retrieved 12 October 2019.
- King, D. Brett; Wertheimer, Michael (1 January 2005). Max Wertheimer and Gestalt Theory. Transaction Publishers. ISBN 978-1-4128-2826-0. Archived from the original on 26 February 2022. Retrieved 2 October 2020.
- Wagemans, Johan; Elder, James H.; Kubovy, Michael; Palmer, Stephen E.; Peterson, Mary A.; Singh, Manish; von der Heydt, Rüdiger (2012). "A century of Gestalt psychology in visual perception: I. Perceptual grouping and figure-ground organization". Psychological Bulletin. 138 (6): 1172–1217. doi:10.1037/a0029333. ISSN 1939-1455. PMC 3482144. PMID 22845751.
- Wertheimer, Max (1912). "Experimentelle Studien über das Sehen von Bewegung" (PDF). Zeitschrift für Psychologie (in German). 61: 161–265. Archived (PDF) from the original on 23 November 2018. Retrieved 14 October 2019. Available in translation as Wertheimer, Max (2012). "Experimental Studies on Seeing Motion". In Spillman, Lothar (ed.). On Perceived Motion and Figural Organization. Michael Wertheimer, K. W. Watkins (trans.). Cambridge, MA: MIT.
- Henle, M (1978). "One man against the Nazis: Wolfgang Köhler" (PDF). American Psychologist. 33 (10): 939–944. doi:10.1037/0003-066x.33.10.939. hdl:11858/00-001M-0000-002B-9E5F-5. Archived from the original on 26 February 2022. Retrieved 16 October 2019.
- Henle, M (1984). "Robert M. Ogden and gestalt psychology in America". Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences. 20 (1): 9–19. doi:10.1002/1520-6696(198401)20:1<9::aid-jhbs2300200103>3.0.co;2-u. PMID 11608590.
- Bocian, B. (2010). Fritz Perls in Berlin 1893–1933: Expressionism – Psychoanalysis – Judaism. EHP – Edition Humanistische Psychology. p. 190. ISBN 978-3-89797-068-7.
- Joe Wysong/Edward Rosenfeld (eds): An Oral History of Gestalt Therapy, Highland, New York 1982, The Gestalt Journal Press, p. 12.
- Barlow, Allen R. (Fall 1981). "Gestalt—Antecedent Influence or Historical Accident". The Gestalt Journal. 4 (2). Retrieved 2 January 2023.
- Mary Henle 1975: Gestalt Psychology and Gestalt Therapy Archived 5 February 2012 at the Wayback Machine; Presidential Address to Division 24 at the meeting of the American Psychological Association, Chicago, September 1975. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences 14, pp 23–32.
- Wertheimer, Max (1938) [Original work published 1924]. "Gestalt theory". In Ellis, Willis D. (ed.). A Source Book of Gestalt Psychology. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul. pp. 2. Retrieved 16 October 2019.
- Köhler, Wolfgang (1971) [Original work published 1930]. "Human perception". In Henle, Mary (ed.). The Selected Papers of Wolfgang Köhler. Liveright. p. 145. ISBN 978-0-87140-253-0.
- Koffka, K. (1935). Principles of Gestalt Psychology. New York: Harcourt, Brace. pp. 176. Retrieved 13 October 2019.
- William Ray Woodward, Robert Sonné Cohen – World views and scientific discipline formation: science studies in the German Democratic Republic: papers from a German-American summer institute, 1988
- Gobet, Fernand (2017), "Entrenchment, Gestalt formation, and chunking", in Schmid, H. J. (ed.), Entrenchment and the psychology of language learning: How we reorganize and adapt linguistic knowledge, American Psychological Association, pp. 245–267, doi:10.1037/15969-012, ISBN 978-3110341300, archived from the original on 26 February 2022, retrieved 14 October 2019
- Steven., Lehar (2003). The World in your Head: A Gestalt View of the Mechanism of Conscious Experience. Mahwah, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers. ISBN 0805841768. OCLC 52051454. Archived from the original on 19 July 2011. Retrieved 19 August 2005.
- "Gestalt Isomorphism". Sharp.bu.edu. Archived from the original on 17 February 2012. Retrieved 6 April 2012.
- Eysenck, Michael W. (2006). Fundamentals of Cognition. Hove, UK: Psychology Press. pp. 62–64. ISBN 978-1-84169-374-3.
- Todorovic, Dejan (2008). "Gestalt Principles". Scholarpedia. 3 (12): 5345. Bibcode:2008SchpJ...3.5345T. doi:10.4249/scholarpedia.5345.
- Koffka, K. (1935). Principles Of Gestalt Psychology. New York: Harcourt, Brace. Retrieved 13 October 2019.
- Sternberg, Robert (2003). Cognitive psychology (3rd ed.). Belmont, CA: Thomson/Wadsworth. ISBN 978-0-15-508535-0. OCLC 883578008.
- Craighead, W. Edward; Nemeroff, Charles B. (19 April 2004). "Gestalt psychology". The Concise Corsini Encyclopedia of Psychology and Behavioral Science. John Wiley & Sons. pp. 401–404. ISBN 9780471220367. Archived from the original on 15 April 2021. Retrieved 2 October 2020.
- Stevenson, Herb. "Emergence: The Gestalt Approach to Change". Unleashing Executive and Orzanizational Potential. Archived from the original on 21 June 2012. Retrieved 7 April 2012.
- Soegaard, Mads. Gestalt Principles of form Perception. Interaction Design. Archived from the original on 10 April 2012. Retrieved 8 April 2012.
- Hamlyn, D. W. (27 March 2017). The Psychology of Perception. doi:10.4324/9781315473291. ISBN 9781315473291.
- Brennan, James F.; Houde, Keith A. (26 October 2017). History and Systems of Psychology. Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/9781316827178. ISBN 9781107178670. S2CID 142935847.
- "Why Your Brain Thinks These Dots Are a Dog". Gizmodo UK. 17 May 2013. Archived from the original on 24 September 2015. Retrieved 23 March 2018.
- Ellis, Willis D. (1999). A source book of Gestalt psychology. Vol. 2. Psychology Press.
- Tenney, James. (1961) 2015. “Meta+Hodos.” In From Scratch: Writings in Music Theory. Edited by Larry Polansky, Lauren Pratt, Robert Wannamaker, and Michael Winter. Urbana: University of Illinois Press. 13–96.
- Mayer, R. E. (1992). Thinking, problem solving, cognition. Second edition. New York: W. H. Freeman and Company.
- Wertheimer, Max (1945). Productive thinking. New York: Harper.
- Kellogg, Ronald T. (2003). Cognitive Psychology (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications. ISBN 0-7619-2130-3.
- Duncker, Karl (1935). Zur Psychologie des produktiven Denkens [The psychology of productive thinking] (in German). Berlin: Julius Springer.
- Bruce, V.; Green, P.; Georgeson, M. (1996). Visual perception: Physiology, psychology and ecology (3rd ed.). LEA. p. 110.
- Carlson, Neil R.; Heth, C. Donald (2010). Psychology: The Science of Behaviour. Ontario: Pearson Education Canada. pp. 20–22.
- Lettvin, J.Y., Maturana, H.R., Pitts, W.H., and McCulloch, W.S. (1961). Two Remarks on the Visual System of the Frog. In Sensory Communication edited by Walter Rosenblith, MIT Press and John Wiley and Sons: New York
- Valentin Fedorovich Turchin – The phenomenon of science – a cybernetic approach to human evolution – Columbia University Press, 1977
- Nauts, Sanne; Langner, Oliver; Huijsmans, Inge; Vonk, Roos; Wigboldus, Daniël H. J. (2014). "Forming Impressions of Personality: A Replication and Review of Evidence for a Primacy-of-Warmth Effect in Impression Formation". Social Psychology. 45 (3): 153–163. doi:10.1027/1864-9335/a000179. hdl:2066/128034. ISSN 1864-9335. S2CID 7693277.
- Sigall, Harold; Ostrove, Nancy (1975). "Beautiful but dangerous: Effects of offender attractiveness and nature of the crime on juridic judgment". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 31 (3): 410–414. doi:10.1037/h0076472. ISSN 0022-3514.
- Jäkel F.; Singh M.; Wichmann F. A.; Herzog, M. H. (2016), "An overview of quantitative approaches in Gestalt perception.", Vision Research, 126: 3–8, doi:10.1016/j.visres.2016.06.004, PMID 27353224
- Yu, Dian; Tam, Derek; Franconeri, Steven L. (2019). "Gestalt similarity groupings are not constructed in parallel". Cognition. 182: 8–13. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2018.08.006. ISSN 0010-0277. PMID 30212653. S2CID 52269830.
- Schultz, Duane (2013). A History of Modern Psychology. Burlington: Elsevier Science. p. 291. ISBN 978-1483270081.
- Murray, David J. (1995). Gestalt Psychology and the Cognitive Revolution. New York: Prentice Hall. ISBN 978-0-13-320714-9.
- Tait, Alex (1 April 2018). "Visual Hierarchy and Layout". Geographic Information Science & Technology Body of Knowledge. 2018 (Q2). University Consortium for Geographic Information Science. doi:10.22224/gistbok/2018.2.4. ISSN 2577-2848.
- Heider, Grace M. (1977). "More about Hull and Koffka". American Psychologist. 32 (5). American Psychological Association: 383. doi:10.1037/0003-066x.32.5.383.a. ISSN 1935-990X.
External links
- Gestalt psychology on Encyclopædia Britannica
- Journal "Gestalt Theory – An International Multidisciplinary Journal" in full text (open source)
- International Society for Gestalt Theory and its Applications – GTA
The neutrality of this article is disputed Relevant discussion may be found on the talk page Please do not remove this message until conditions to do so are met November 2016 Learn how and when to remove this message Gestalt psychology gestaltism or configurationism is a school of psychology and a theory of perception that emphasises the processing of entire patterns and configurations and not merely individual components It emerged in the early twentieth century in Austria and Germany as a rejection of basic principles of Wilhelm Wundt s and Edward Titchener s elementalist and structuralist psychology Gestalt psychology is often associated with the adage The whole is greater than the sum of its parts In Gestalt theory information is perceived as wholes rather than disparate parts which are then processed summatively As used in Gestalt psychology the German word Gestalt ɡ e ˈ ʃ t ae l t ˈ ʃ t ɑː l t ge SHTA H LT German ɡeˈʃtalt meaning form is interpreted as pattern or configuration It differs from Gestalt therapy which is only peripherally linked to Gestalt psychology Origin and historyMax Wertheimer Kurt Koffka and Wolfgang Kohler founded Gestalt psychology in the early 20th century 113 116 The dominant view in psychology at the time was structuralism exemplified by the work of Hermann von Helmholtz Wilhelm Wundt and Edward B Titchener 3 Structuralism was rooted firmly in British empiricism 3 and was based on three closely interrelated theories atomism also known as elementalism 3 the view that all knowledge even complex abstract ideas is built from simple elementary constituents sensationalism the view that the simplest constituents the atoms of thought are elementary sense impressions associationism the view that more complex ideas arise from the association of simpler ideas 3 Together these three theories give rise to the view that the mind constructs all perceptions and abstract thoughts strictly from lower level sensations which are related solely by being associated closely in space and time The Gestaltists took issue with the widespread atomistic view that the aim of psychology should be to break consciousness down into putative basic elements In contrast the Gestalt psychologists believed that breaking psychological phenomena down into smaller parts would not lead to understanding psychology 13 Instead they viewed psychological phenomena as organized structured wholes 13 They argued that the psychological whole has priority and that the parts are defined by the structure of the whole rather than the other way round Gestalt theories of perception are based on human nature being inclined to understand objects as an entire structure rather than the sum of its parts Wertheimer had been a student of Austrian philosopher Christian von Ehrenfels a member of the School of Brentano Von Ehrenfels introduced the concept of Gestalt to philosophy and psychology in 1890 before the advent of Gestalt psychology as such Von Ehrenfels observed that a perceptual experience such as perceiving a melody or a shape is more than the sum of its sensory components He claimed that in addition to the sensory elements of the perception there is something additional that is an element in its own right despite in some sense being derived from the organization of the component sensory elements He called it Gestalt qualitat or form quality It is this Gestalt qualitat that according to von Ehrenfels allows a tune to be transposed to a new key using completely different notes while still retaining its identity The idea of a Gestalt qualitat has roots in theories by David Hume Johann Wolfgang von Goethe Immanuel Kant David Hartley and Ernst Mach Both von Ehrenfels and Edmund Husserl seem to have been inspired by Mach s work Beitrage zur Analyse der Empfindungen Contributions to the Analysis of Sensations 1886 in formulating their very similar concepts of gestalt and figural moment respectively By 1914 the first published references to Gestalt theory could be found in a footnote of Gabriele von Wartensleben s application of Gestalt theory to personality She was a student at Frankfurt Academy for Social Sciences who interacted deeply with Wertheimer and Kohler Through a series of experiments Wertheimer discovered that a person observing a pair of alternating bars of light can under the right conditions experience the illusion of movement between one location and the other He noted that this was a perception of motion absent any moving object That is it was pure phenomenal motion He dubbed it phi phenomenal motion Wertheimer s publication of these results in 1912 marks the beginning of Gestalt psychology In comparison to von Ehrenfels and others who had used the term gestalt earlier in various ways Wertheimer s unique contribution was to insist that the gestalt is perceptually primary The gestalt defines the parts from which it is composed rather than being a secondary quality that emerges from those parts Wertheimer took the more radical position that one hears the melody first and only then may perceptually divide it up into notes Similarly in vision one sees the form of the circle first with its apprehension not mediated by a process of part summation Only after this primary apprehension might one notice that it is made up of lines or dots or stars The two men who served as Wertheimer s subjects in the phi experiments were Kohler and Koffka Kohler was an expert in physical acoustics having studied under physicist Max Planck but had taken his degree in psychology under Carl Stumpf Koffka was also a student of Stumpf s having studied movement phenomena and psychological aspects of rhythm In 1917 Kohler published the results of four years of research on learning in chimpanzees Kohler showed contrary to the claims of most other learning theorists that animals can learn by sudden insight into the structure of a problem over and above the associative and incremental manner of learning that Ivan Pavlov and Edward Lee Thorndike had demonstrated with dogs and cats respectively In 1921 Koffka published a Gestalt oriented text on developmental psychology Growth of the Mind With the help of American psychologist Robert Ogden Koffka introduced the Gestalt point of view to an American audience in 1922 by way of a paper in Psychological Bulletin It contains criticisms of then current explanations of a number of problems of perception and the alternatives offered by the Gestalt school Koffka moved to the United States in 1924 eventually settling at Smith College in 1927 In 1935 Koffka published his Principles of Gestalt Psychology This textbook laid out the Gestalt vision of the scientific enterprise as a whole Science he said is not the simple accumulation of facts What makes research scientific is the incorporation of facts into a theoretical structure The goal of the Gestaltists was to integrate the facts of inanimate nature life and mind into a single scientific structure This meant that science would have to accommodate not only what Koffka called the quantitative facts of physical science but the facts of two other scientific categories questions of order and questions of Sinn a German word which has been variously translated as significance value and meaning Without incorporating the meaning of experience and behavior Koffka believed that science would doom itself to trivialities in its investigation of human beings Having survived the Nazis up to the mid 1930s all the core members of the Gestalt movement were forced out of Germany to the United States by 1935 Kohler published another book Dynamics in Psychology in 1940 but thereafter the Gestalt movement suffered a series of setbacks Koffka died in 1941 and Wertheimer in 1943 Wertheimer s long awaited book on mathematical problem solving Productive Thinking was published posthumously in 1945 but Kohler was left to guide the movement without his two long time colleagues Gestalt therapy Gestalt psychology differs from Gestalt therapy which is only peripherally linked to Gestalt psychology The founders of Gestalt therapy Fritz and Laura Perls had worked with Kurt Goldstein a neurologist who had applied principles of Gestalt psychology to the functioning of the organism Laura Perls had been a Gestalt psychologist before she became a psychoanalyst and before she began developing Gestalt therapy together with Fritz Perls The extent to which Gestalt psychology influenced Gestalt therapy is disputed On one hand Laura Perls preferred not to use the term Gestalt to name the emerging new therapy because she thought that the Gestalt psychologists would object to it on the other hand Fritz and Laura Perls clearly adopted some of Goldstein s work Mary Henle noted in her presidential address to Division 24 at the meeting of the American Psychological Association What Perls has done has been to take a few terms from Gestalt psychology stretch their meaning beyond recognition mix them with notions often unclear and often incompatible from the depth psychologies existentialism and common sense and he has called the whole mixture gestalt therapy His work has no substantive relation to scientific Gestalt psychology To use his own language Fritz Perls has done his thing whatever it is it is not Gestalt psychology One form of psychotherapy that unlike Gestalt therapy is actually consistently based on Gestalt psychology is Gestalt theoretical psychotherapy Theoretical framework and methodologyThe Gestalt psychologists practiced a set of theoretical and methodological principles that attempted to redefine the approach to psychological research This is in contrast to investigations developed at the beginning of the 20th century based on traditional scientific methodology which divided the object of study into a set of elements that could be analyzed separately with the objective of reducing the complexity of this object The principle of totality asserts that conscious experience must be considered globally by taking into account all the physical and mental aspects of the individual simultaneously because the nature of the mind demands that each component be considered as part of a system of dynamic relationships Thus holism as fundamental aspect of Gestalt psychology Moreover the perception of the nature of a part depends upon the whole in which it is embedded The maxim that the whole is more than the sum of its parts is not a precise description of the Gestaltist view Rather as Koffka writes The whole is something else than the sum of its parts because summing is a meaningless procedure whereas the whole part relationship is meaningful The principle of psychophysical isomorphism hypothesizes that there is a correlation between conscious experience and cerebral activity Based on the principles phenomenon experimental analysis was derived which asserts that any psychological research should take phenomena as a starting point and not be solely focused on sensory qualities A related principle is that of the biotic experiment which establishes the need to conduct real experiments that sharply contrasted with and opposed classic laboratory experiments This signified experimenting in natural situations developed in real conditions in which it would be possible to reproduce with higher fidelity what would be habitual for a subject PrinciplesThe Gestaltists were the first to document and demonstrate empirically many facts about perception including facts about the perception of movement the perception of contour perceptual constancy and perceptual illusions Wertheimer s discovery of the phi phenomenon is one example of such a contribution Properties The key principles of gestalt systems are emergence reification multistability and invariance These principles are not necessarily separable modules to model individually but they could be different aspects of a single unified dynamic mechanism Reification Reification Reification is the constructive or generative aspect of perception by which the experienced object of perception contains more explicit spatial information than the sensory stimulus on which it is based For instance a triangle is perceived in picture A though no triangle is there In pictures B and D the eye recognizes disparate shapes as belonging to a single shape in C a complete three dimensional shape is seen where in actuality no such thing is drawn Reification can be explained by progress in the study of illusory contours which are treated by the visual system as real contours Multistability The Necker cube and the Rubin vase two examples of multistability Multistability or multistable perception is the tendency of ambiguous perceptual experiences to pop back and forth between two or more alternative interpretations This is seen for example in the Necker cube and Rubin s Figure Vase illusion Other examples include the three legged blivet artist M C Escher s artwork and the appearance of flashing marquee lights moving first one direction and then suddenly the other Invariance Invariance Invariance is the property of perception whereby simple geometrical objects are recognized independent of rotation translation and scale as well as several other variations such as elastic deformations different lighting and different component features For example the objects in A in the figure are all immediately recognized as the same basic shape which is immediately distinguishable from the forms in B They are even recognized despite perspective and elastic deformations as in C and when depicted using different graphic elements as in D Computational theories of vision such as those by David Marr have provided alternate explanations of how perceived objects are classified Perceptual organisation formsPerceptual grouping Like figure ground organization perceptual grouping sometimes called perceptual segregation is a form of perceptual organization Perceptual grouping is the process that determines how organisms perceive some parts of their perceptual fields as being more related than others using such information for object detection The Gestaltists were the first psychologists to systematically study perceptual grouping According to Gestalt psychologists the fundamental principle of perceptual grouping is the law of Pragnanz also known as the law of good Gestalt Pragnanz is a German word that directly translates to pithiness and implies salience conciseness and orderliness The law of Pragnanz says that people tend to experience things as regular orderly symmetrical and simple Gestalt psychologists attempted to discover refinements of the law of Pragnanz which involved writing down laws that predict the interpretation of sensation Wertheimer defined a few principles that explain the ways humans perceive objects based on similarity proximity and continuity Law of proximity Law of proximity The law of proximity states that when an individual perceives an assortment of objects they perceive objects that are close to each other as forming a group For example in the figure illustrating the law of proximity there are 72 circles but we perceive the collection of circles in groups Specifically we perceive that there is a group of 36 circles on the left side of the image and three groups of 12 circles on the right side of the image This law is often used in advertising logos to emphasize which aspects of events are associated Law of similarity Law of similarity The law of similarity states that elements within an assortment of objects are perceptually grouped together if they are similar to each other This similarity can occur in the form of shape colour shading or other qualities For example the figure illustrating the law of similarity portrays 36 circles all equal distance apart from one another forming a square In this depiction 18 of the circles are shaded dark and 18 of the circles are shaded light We perceive the dark circles as grouped together and the light circles as grouped together forming six horizontal lines within the square of circles This perception of lines is due to the law of similarity Law of closure Law of closure Gestalt psychologists believed that humans tend to perceive objects as complete rather than focusing on the gaps that the object might contain For example a circle has good Gestalt in terms of completeness However we will also perceive an incomplete circle as a complete circle That tendency to complete shapes and figures is called closure The law of closure states that individuals perceive objects such as shapes letters pictures etc as being whole when they are not complete Specifically when parts of a whole picture are missing our perception fills in the visual gap Research shows that the reason the mind completes a regular figure that is not perceived through sensation is to increase the regularity of surrounding stimuli For example the figure that depicts the law of closure portrays what we perceive as a circle on the left side of the image and a rectangle on the right side of the image However gaps are present in the shapes If the law of closure did not exist the image would depict an assortment of different lines with different lengths rotations and curvatures but with the law of closure we perceptually combine the lines into whole shapes Law of symmetry Law of symmetry The law of symmetry states that the mind perceives objects as being symmetrical and forming around a center point It is perceptually pleasing to divide objects into an even number of symmetrical parts Therefore when two symmetrical elements are unconnected the mind perceptually connects them to form a coherent shape Similarities between symmetrical objects increase the likelihood that objects are grouped to form a combined symmetrical object For example the figure depicting the law of symmetry shows a configuration of square and curled brackets When the image is perceived we tend to observe three pairs of symmetrical brackets rather than six individual brackets Law of common fate The law of common fate states that objects are perceived as lines that move along the smoothest path Experiments using the visual sensory modality found that the movement of elements of an object produces paths that individuals perceive that the objects are on We perceive elements of objects to have trends of motion which indicate the path that the object is on The law of continuity implies the grouping together of objects that have the same trend of motion and are therefore on the same path For example if there is an array of dots and half the dots are moving upward while the other half are moving downward we would perceive the upward moving dots and the downward moving dots as two distinct units Law of continuity Law of continuity The law of continuity also known as the law of good continuation states that elements of objects tend to be grouped together and therefore integrated into perceptual wholes if they are aligned within an object In cases where there is an intersection between objects individuals tend to perceive the two objects as two single uninterrupted entities Stimuli remain distinct even with overlap We are less likely to group elements with sharp abrupt directional changes as being one object For example the figure depicting the law of continuity shows a configuration of two crossed keys When the image is perceived we tend to perceive the key in the background as a single uninterrupted key instead of two separate halves of a key Law of past experience The law of past experience implies that under some circumstances visual stimuli are categorized according to past experience If objects tend to be observed within close proximity or small temporal intervals the objects are more likely to be perceived together For example the English language contains 26 letters that are grouped to form words using a set of rules If an individual reads an English word they have never seen they use the law of past experience to interpret the letters L and I as two letters beside each other rather than using the law of closure to combine the letters and interpret the object as an uppercase U Music An example of the Gestalt movement in effect as it is both a process and result is a music sequence People are able to recognise a sequence of perhaps six or seven notes despite them being transposed into a different tuning or key An early theory of gestalt grouping principles in music was composer theorist James Tenney s Meta Hodos 1961 Auditory Scene Analysis as developed by Albert Bregman further extends a gestalt approach to the analysis of sound perception Figure ground organization Figure ground organization is a form of perceptual organization which interprets perceptual elements in terms of their shapes and relative locations in the layout of surfaces in the 3 D world Figure ground organization structures the perceptual field into a figure standing out at the front of the perceptual field and a background receding behind the figure Pioneering work on figure ground organization was carried out by the Danish psychologist Edgar Rubin The Gestalt psychologists demonstrated that people tend to perceive as figures those parts of our perceptual fields that are convex symmetric small and enclosed Problem solving and insightGestalt psychology contributed to the scientific study of problem solving In fact the early experimental work of the Gestaltists in Germany marks the beginning of the scientific study of problem solving Later this experimental work continued through the 1960s and early 1970s with research conducted on relatively simple laboratory tasks of problem solving Max Wertheimer distinguished two kinds of thinking productive thinking and reproductive thinking 456 361 Productive thinking is solving a problem based on insight a quick creative unplanned response to situations and environmental interaction Reproductive thinking is solving a problem deliberately based on previous experience and knowledge Reproductive thinking proceeds algorithmically a problem solver reproduces a series of steps from memory knowing that they will lead to a solution or by trial and error 361 Karl Duncker another Gestalt psychologist who studied problem solving 370 coined the term functional fixedness for describing the difficulties in both visual perception and problem solving that arise from the fact that one element of a whole situation already has a fixed function that has to be changed in order to perceive something or find the solution to a problem LegacyGestalt psychology struggled to precisely define terms like Pragnanz to make specific behavioural predictions and to articulate testable models of underlying neural mechanisms It was criticized as being merely descriptive These shortcomings led by the mid 20th century to growing dissatisfaction with Gestaltism and a subsequent decline in its impact on psychology Despite this decline Gestalt psychology has formed the basis of much further research into the perception of patterns and objects and of research into behaviour thinking problem solving and psychopathology Support from cybernetics and neurology In the 1940s and 1950s laboratory research in neurology and what became known as cybernetics on the mechanism of frogs eyes indicate that perception of gestalts in particular gestalts in motion is perhaps more primitive and fundamental than seeing as such A frog hunts on land by vision He has no fovea or region of greatest acuity in vision upon which he must centre a part of the image The frog does not seem to see or at any rate is not concerned with the detail of stationary parts of the world around him He will starve to death surrounded by food if it is not moving His choice of food is determined only by size and movement He will leap to capture any object the size of an insect or worm providing it moves like one He can be fooled easily not only by a piece of dangled meat but by any moving small object He does remember a moving thing provided it stays within his field of vision and he is not distracted The lowest level concepts related to visual perception for a human being probably differ little from the concepts of a frog In any case the structure of the retina in mammals and in human beings is the same as in amphibians The phenomenon of distortion of perception of an image stabilised on the retina gives some idea of the concepts of the subsequent levels of the hierarchy This is a very interesting phenomenon When a person looks at an immobile object fixes it with his eyes the eyeballs do not remain absolutely immobile they make small involuntary movements As a result the image of the object on the retina is constantly in motion slowly drifting and jumping back to the point of maximum sensitivity The image marks time in the vicinity of this point Use in contemporary social psychology The halo effect can be explained through the application of Gestalt theories to social information processing The constructive theories of social cognition are applied to the expectations of individuals They have been perceived in this manner and the person judging the individual is continuing to view them in this positive manner Gestalt s theories of perception enforces that individual s tendency to perceive actions and characteristics as a whole rather than isolated parts therefore humans are inclined to build a coherent and consistent impression of objects and behaviors in order to achieve an acceptable shape and form The halo effect is what forms patterns for individuals the halo effect being classified as a cognitive bias which occurs during impression formation The halo effect can also be altered by physical characteristics social status and many other characteristics As well the halo effect can have real repercussions on the individual s perception of reality either negatively or positively meaning to construct negative or positive images about other individuals or situations something that could lead to self fulfilling prophesies stereotyping or even discrimination Contemporary cognitive and perceptual psychology Some of the central criticisms of Gestaltism are based on the preference Gestaltists are deemed to have for theory over data and a lack of quantitative research supporting Gestalt ideas This is not necessarily a fair criticism as highlighted by a recent collection of quantitative research on Gestalt perception Researchers continue to test hypotheses about the mechanisms underlying Gestalt principles such as the principle of similarity Other important criticisms concern the lack of definition and support for the many physiological assumptions made by gestaltists and lack of theoretical coherence in modern Gestalt psychology In some scholarly communities such as cognitive psychology and computational neuroscience gestalt theories of perception are criticized for being descriptive rather than explanatory in nature For this reason they are viewed by some as redundant or uninformative For example a textbook on visual perception states that The physiological theory of the gestaltists has fallen by the wayside leaving us with a set of descriptive principles but without a model of perceptual processing Indeed some of their laws of perceptual organisation today sound vague and inadequate What is meant by a good or simple shape for example One historian of psychology David J Murray has argued that Gestalt psychologists first discovered many principles later championed by cognitive psychology including schemas and prototypes Another psychologist has argued that the Gestalt psychologists made a lasting contribution by showing how the study of illusions can help scientists understand essential aspects of how the visual system normally functions not merely how it breaks down 16 Use in design Several grouping principles are employed in this map similarity allows the reader to selectively isolate cities rivers or state boundaries closure allows the dashed boundary lines to be perceived as continuous borders proximity makes the collection of river segments appear as a single watershed and continuity helps the reader see whole states even when boundaries are obscured under rivers The gestalt laws are used in several visual design fields such as user interface design and cartography The laws of similarity and proximity can for example be used as guides for placing radio buttons They may also be used in designing computers and software for more intuitive human use Examples include the design and layout of a desktop s shortcuts in rows and columns In map design principles of Pragnanz or grouping are crucial for implying a conceptual order to the portrayed geographic features thus facilitating the intended use of the map The Law of Similarity is employed by selecting similar map symbols for similar kinds of features or features with similar properties the Law of Proximity is crucial to identifying geographic patterns and regions and the Laws of Closure and Continuity allow users to recognize features that may be obscured by other features such as when a road goes over a river See alsoAugusto Garau Amodal perception Cognitive grammar Egregore Gestaltzerfall Graz School Hans Wallach Hermann Friedmann James J Gibson James Tenney Laws of association Mereology Optical illusion Pal Schiller Harkai Pattern recognition machine learning Pattern recognition psychology Phenomenology Principles of grouping Rudolf Arnheim Solomon Asch Structural information theory Topological data analysis Wolfgang Metzger Vera Felicidade de Almeida CamposNotesFor more on the history of Gestalt psychology see Ash M G 1995 Gestalt psychology in German culture 1890 1967 Holism and the quest for objectivity Cambridge England Cambridge University Press For example Duncker Karl 1935 Zur Psychologie des produktiven Denkens The psychology of productive thinking in German Berlin Julius Springer For example Duncker s X ray problem Ewert amp Lambert s disk problem in 1932 later known as Tower of Hanoi ReferencesMather George 2006 Foundations of Perception Psychology Press p 32 ISBN 978 0 86377 834 6 Gestalt psychology Encyclopaedia Britannica Retrieved 2 January 2023 Baker David B ed 13 January 2012 The Oxford Handbook of the History of Psychology Global Perspectives Oxford University Press USA p 576 ISBN 978 0 19 536655 6 Wells John 3 April 2008 Longman Pronunciation Dictionary 3rd ed Pearson Longman ISBN 978 1 4058 8118 0 Definition of gestalt Dictionary com www dictionary com Retrieved 2 January 2023 Gestalt The Columbia Encyclopedia Columbia University Press 2018 ISBN 9781786848468 Archived from the original on 22 July 2019 Retrieved 1 August 2019 Gestalt psychology Britannica Concise Encyclopedia Encyclopaedia Britannica Inc 1 May 2008 p 756 ISBN 9781593394929 Archived from the original on 15 April 2021 Retrieved 2 October 2020 Sternberg Robert J Sternberg Karin 2012 Cognitive Psychology 6th ed Belmont CA Cengage Learning ISBN 978 1 133 31391 5 Wagemans Johan Feldman Jacob Gepshtein Sergei Kimchi Ruth Pomerantz James R van der Helm Peter A van Leeuwen Cees 2012 A century of Gestalt psychology in visual perception II Conceptual and theoretical foundations Psychological Bulletin 138 6 1218 1252 doi 10 1037 a0029334 ISSN 1939 1455 PMC 3728284 PMID 22845750 Kolers Paul A 1972 Aspects of Motion Perception International Series of Monographs in Experimental Psychology New York Pergamon ISBN 978 1 4831 7113 5 Hamlyn D W 1957 The Psychology of Perception A Philosophical Examination of Gestalt Theory and Derivative Theories of Perception eBook ed London Routledge pp 88 89 ISBN 978 1 315 47329 1 Archived from the original on 21 October 2020 Retrieved 19 October 2019 Verstegen Ian 2010 Gestalt Psychology The Corsini Encyclopedia of Psychology pp 1 3 doi 10 1002 9780470479216 corpsy0386 ISBN 9780470479216 Pohl Rudiger F 22 July 2016 Cognitive Illusions Intriguing Phenomena in Judgement Thinking and Memory Psychology Press ISBN 9781317448280 Archived from the original on 15 April 2021 Retrieved 2 October 2020 Smith Barry 1988 Gestalt Theory An Essay in Philosophy In Smith Barry ed Foundations of Gestalt Theory Vienna Philosophia Verlag pp 11 81 Archived from the original PDF on 22 February 2012 Retrieved 12 October 2019 King D Brett Wertheimer Michael 1 January 2005 Max Wertheimer and Gestalt Theory Transaction Publishers ISBN 978 1 4128 2826 0 Archived from the original on 26 February 2022 Retrieved 2 October 2020 Wagemans Johan Elder James H Kubovy Michael Palmer Stephen E Peterson Mary A Singh Manish von der Heydt Rudiger 2012 A century of Gestalt psychology in visual perception I Perceptual grouping and figure ground organization Psychological Bulletin 138 6 1172 1217 doi 10 1037 a0029333 ISSN 1939 1455 PMC 3482144 PMID 22845751 Wertheimer Max 1912 Experimentelle Studien uber das Sehen von Bewegung PDF Zeitschrift fur Psychologie in German 61 161 265 Archived PDF from the original on 23 November 2018 Retrieved 14 October 2019 Available in translation as Wertheimer Max 2012 Experimental Studies on Seeing Motion In Spillman Lothar ed On Perceived Motion and Figural Organization Michael Wertheimer K W Watkins trans Cambridge MA MIT Henle M 1978 One man against the Nazis Wolfgang Kohler PDF American Psychologist 33 10 939 944 doi 10 1037 0003 066x 33 10 939 hdl 11858 00 001M 0000 002B 9E5F 5 Archived from the original on 26 February 2022 Retrieved 16 October 2019 Henle M 1984 Robert M Ogden and gestalt psychology in America Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences 20 1 9 19 doi 10 1002 1520 6696 198401 20 1 lt 9 aid jhbs2300200103 gt 3 0 co 2 u PMID 11608590 Bocian B 2010 Fritz Perls in Berlin 1893 1933 Expressionism Psychoanalysis Judaism EHP Edition Humanistische Psychology p 190 ISBN 978 3 89797 068 7 Joe Wysong Edward Rosenfeld eds An Oral History of Gestalt Therapy Highland New York 1982 The Gestalt Journal Press p 12 Barlow Allen R Fall 1981 Gestalt Antecedent Influence or Historical Accident The Gestalt Journal 4 2 Retrieved 2 January 2023 Mary Henle 1975 Gestalt Psychology and Gestalt Therapy Archived 5 February 2012 at the Wayback Machine Presidential Address to Division 24 at the meeting of the American Psychological Association Chicago September 1975 Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences 14 pp 23 32 Wertheimer Max 1938 Original work published 1924 Gestalt theory In Ellis Willis D ed A Source Book of Gestalt Psychology London Routledge amp Kegan Paul pp 2 Retrieved 16 October 2019 Kohler Wolfgang 1971 Original work published 1930 Human perception In Henle Mary ed The Selected Papers of Wolfgang Kohler Liveright p 145 ISBN 978 0 87140 253 0 Koffka K 1935 Principles of Gestalt Psychology New York Harcourt Brace pp 176 Retrieved 13 October 2019 William Ray Woodward Robert Sonne Cohen World views and scientific discipline formation science studies in the German Democratic Republic papers from a German American summer institute 1988 Gobet Fernand 2017 Entrenchment Gestalt formation and chunking in Schmid H J ed Entrenchment and the psychology of language learning How we reorganize and adapt linguistic knowledge American Psychological Association pp 245 267 doi 10 1037 15969 012 ISBN 978 3110341300 archived from the original on 26 February 2022 retrieved 14 October 2019 Steven Lehar 2003 The World in your Head A Gestalt View of the Mechanism of Conscious Experience Mahwah N J Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers ISBN 0805841768 OCLC 52051454 Archived from the original on 19 July 2011 Retrieved 19 August 2005 Gestalt Isomorphism Sharp bu edu Archived from the original on 17 February 2012 Retrieved 6 April 2012 Eysenck Michael W 2006 Fundamentals of Cognition Hove UK Psychology Press pp 62 64 ISBN 978 1 84169 374 3 Todorovic Dejan 2008 Gestalt Principles Scholarpedia 3 12 5345 Bibcode 2008SchpJ 3 5345T doi 10 4249 scholarpedia 5345 Koffka K 1935 Principles Of Gestalt Psychology New York Harcourt Brace Retrieved 13 October 2019 Sternberg Robert 2003 Cognitive psychology 3rd ed Belmont CA Thomson Wadsworth ISBN 978 0 15 508535 0 OCLC 883578008 Craighead W Edward Nemeroff Charles B 19 April 2004 Gestalt psychology The Concise Corsini Encyclopedia of Psychology and Behavioral Science John Wiley amp Sons pp 401 404 ISBN 9780471220367 Archived from the original on 15 April 2021 Retrieved 2 October 2020 Stevenson Herb Emergence The Gestalt Approach to Change Unleashing Executive and Orzanizational Potential Archived from the original on 21 June 2012 Retrieved 7 April 2012 Soegaard Mads Gestalt Principles of form Perception Interaction Design Archived from the original on 10 April 2012 Retrieved 8 April 2012 Hamlyn D W 27 March 2017 The Psychology of Perception doi 10 4324 9781315473291 ISBN 9781315473291 Brennan James F Houde Keith A 26 October 2017 History and Systems of Psychology Cambridge University Press doi 10 1017 9781316827178 ISBN 9781107178670 S2CID 142935847 Why Your Brain Thinks These Dots Are a Dog Gizmodo UK 17 May 2013 Archived from the original on 24 September 2015 Retrieved 23 March 2018 Ellis Willis D 1999 A source book of Gestalt psychology Vol 2 Psychology Press Tenney James 1961 2015 Meta Hodos In From Scratch Writings in Music Theory Edited by Larry Polansky Lauren Pratt Robert Wannamaker and Michael Winter Urbana University of Illinois Press 13 96 Mayer R E 1992 Thinking problem solving cognition Second edition New York W H Freeman and Company Wertheimer Max 1945 Productive thinking New York Harper Kellogg Ronald T 2003 Cognitive Psychology 2nd ed Thousand Oaks CA SAGE Publications ISBN 0 7619 2130 3 Duncker Karl 1935 Zur Psychologie des produktiven Denkens The psychology of productive thinking in German Berlin Julius Springer Bruce V Green P Georgeson M 1996 Visual perception Physiology psychology and ecology 3rd ed LEA p 110 Carlson Neil R Heth C Donald 2010 Psychology The Science of Behaviour Ontario Pearson Education Canada pp 20 22 Lettvin J Y Maturana H R Pitts W H and McCulloch W S 1961 Two Remarks on the Visual System of the Frog In Sensory Communication edited by Walter Rosenblith MIT Press and John Wiley and Sons New York Valentin Fedorovich Turchin The phenomenon of science a cybernetic approach to human evolution Columbia University Press 1977 Nauts Sanne Langner Oliver Huijsmans Inge Vonk Roos Wigboldus Daniel H J 2014 Forming Impressions of Personality A Replication and Review of Evidence for a Primacy of Warmth Effect in Impression Formation Social Psychology 45 3 153 163 doi 10 1027 1864 9335 a000179 hdl 2066 128034 ISSN 1864 9335 S2CID 7693277 Sigall Harold Ostrove Nancy 1975 Beautiful but dangerous Effects of offender attractiveness and nature of the crime on juridic judgment Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 31 3 410 414 doi 10 1037 h0076472 ISSN 0022 3514 Jakel F Singh M Wichmann F A Herzog M H 2016 An overview of quantitative approaches in Gestalt perception Vision Research 126 3 8 doi 10 1016 j visres 2016 06 004 PMID 27353224 Yu Dian Tam Derek Franconeri Steven L 2019 Gestalt similarity groupings are not constructed in parallel Cognition 182 8 13 doi 10 1016 j cognition 2018 08 006 ISSN 0010 0277 PMID 30212653 S2CID 52269830 Schultz Duane 2013 A History of Modern Psychology Burlington Elsevier Science p 291 ISBN 978 1483270081 Murray David J 1995 Gestalt Psychology and the Cognitive Revolution New York Prentice Hall ISBN 978 0 13 320714 9 Tait Alex 1 April 2018 Visual Hierarchy and Layout Geographic Information Science amp Technology Body of Knowledge 2018 Q2 University Consortium for Geographic Information Science doi 10 22224 gistbok 2018 2 4 ISSN 2577 2848 Heider Grace M 1977 More about Hull and Koffka American Psychologist 32 5 American Psychological Association 383 doi 10 1037 0003 066x 32 5 383 a ISSN 1935 990X External linksPsychology portalLibrary resources about Gestalt psychology Resources in your library Gestalt psychology on Encyclopaedia Britannica Journal Gestalt Theory An International Multidisciplinary Journal in full text open source International Society for Gestalt Theory and its Applications GTA