![Consonant cluster](https://www.english.nina.az/image-resize/1600/900/web/wikipedia.jpg)
This article has multiple issues. Please help improve it or discuss these issues on the talk page. (Learn how and when to remove these messages)
|
In linguistics, a consonant cluster, consonant sequence or consonant compound, is a group of consonants which have no intervening vowel. In English, for example, the groups /spl/ and /ts/ are consonant clusters in the word splits. In the education field it is variously called a consonant cluster or a consonant blend.
Some linguists[who?] argue that the term can be properly applied only to those consonant clusters that occur within one syllable. Others claim that the concept is more useful when it includes consonant sequences across syllable boundaries. According to the former definition, the longest consonant clusters in the word extra would be /ks/ and /tr/, whereas the latter allows /kstr/, which is phonetically [kst̠ɹ̠̊˔ʷ] in some accents.
Phonotactics
This section has multiple issues. Please help improve it or discuss these issues on the talk page. (Learn how and when to remove these messages)
|
Each language has an associated set of phonotactic constraints. Languages' phonotactics differ as to what consonant clusters they permit. Many languages are more restrictive than English in terms of consonant clusters, and some forbid consonant clusters entirely.
For example, Hawaiian, like most Oceanic languages, forbids consonant clusters entirely. Japanese is almost as strict, but allows a sequence of a nasal consonant plus another consonant, as in Honshū [hoꜜɰ̃ɕɯː] (the name of the largest island of Japan). It also permits geminate /kk/, /pp/, /ss/, and /tt/. However, palatalized consonants, such as [kʲ] in Tōkyō [toːkʲoː], are single consonants.
Standard Arabic forbids initial consonant clusters and more than two consecutive consonants in other positions, as do most other Semitic languages, although Modern Israeli Hebrew permits initial two-consonant clusters (e.g. pkak "cap"; dlaat "pumpkin"), and Moroccan Arabic, under Berber influence, allows strings of several consonants.
Like most Mon–Khmer languages, Khmer permits only initial consonant clusters with up to three consonants in a row per syllable. Finnish has initial consonant clusters natively only on South-Western dialects and on foreign loans, and only clusters of three inside the word are allowed. Most spoken languages and dialects, however, are more permissive. In Burmese, consonant clusters of only up to three consonants (the initial and two medials—two written forms of /-j-/, /-w-/) at the initial onset are allowed in writing and only two (the initial and one medial) are pronounced; these clusters are restricted to certain letters. Some Burmese dialects allow for clusters of up to four consonants (with the addition of the /-l-/ medial, which can combine with the above-mentioned medials).
At the other end of the scale, the Kartvelian languages of Georgia are drastically more permissive of consonant clustering. Clusters in Georgian of four, five or six consonants are not unusual—for instance, /brtʼqʼɛli/ (flat), /mt͡sʼvrtnɛli/ (trainer) and /prt͡skvna/ (peeling)—and if grammatical affixes are used, it allows an eight-consonant cluster: /ɡvbrdɣvnis/ (he's plucking us), /gvprt͡skvni/ (you peel us). Consonants cannot appear as syllable nuclei in Georgian, so this syllable is analysed as CCCCCCCCVC. Many Slavic languages may manifest almost as formidable numbers of consecutive consonants, such as in the Czech tongue twister Strč prst skrz krk (pronounced [str̩tʃ pr̩st skr̩s kr̩k] ), meaning 'stick a finger through the neck', the Slovak words štvrť /ʃtvr̩c/ ("quarter"), and žblnknutie /ʒbl̩ŋknucɪɛ̯/ ("clunk"; "flop"), and the Slovene word skrbstvo /skrbstʋo/ ("welfare"). However, the liquid consonants /r/ and /l/ can form syllable nuclei in West and South Slavic languages and behave phonologically as vowels in this case.
An example of a true initial cluster is the Polish word wszczniesz (/fʂt͡ʂɲɛʂ/ ("you will initiate"). In the Serbo-Croatian word opskrbljivanje /ɔpskr̩bʎiʋaɲɛ/ ("victualling") the ⟨lj⟩ and ⟨nj⟩ are digraphs representing single consonants: [ʎ] and [ɲ], respectively. In Dutch, clusters of six or even seven consonants are possible (e.g. angstschreeuw ("a scream of fear"), slechtstschrijvend ("writing the worst") and zachtstschrijdend ("treading the most softly")).
Some Salishan languages exhibit long words with no vowels at all, such as the Nuxálk word /xɬpʼχʷɬtʰɬpʰɬːskʷʰt͡sʼ/: he had had in his possession a bunchberry plant. It is extremely difficult to accurately classify which of these consonants may be acting as the syllable nucleus, and these languages challenge classical notions of exactly what constitutes a syllable. The same problem is encountered in the Northern Berber languages.
There has been a trend to reduce and simplify consonant clusters in the Mainland Southeast Asia linguistic area, such as Chinese and Vietnamese. Old Chinese was known to contain additional medials such as /r/ and/or /l/, which yielded retroflexion in Middle Chinese and today's Mandarin Chinese. The word 江, read /tɕiɑŋ˥/ in Mandarin and /kɔːŋ˥⁻˥˧/in Cantonese, is reconstructed as *klong or *krung in Old Chinese by Sinologists like Zhengzhang Shangfang, William H. Baxter, and Laurent Sagart. Additionally, initial clusters such as "tk" and "sn" were analysed in recent reconstructions of Old Chinese, and some were developed as palatalised sibilants. Similarly, in Thai, words with initial consonant clusters are commonly reduced in colloquial speech to pronounce only the initial consonant, such as the pronunciation of the word ครับ reducing from /kʰrap̚˦˥/ to /kʰap̚˦˥/.
Another element of consonant clusters in Old Chinese was analysed in coda and post-coda position. Some "departing tone" syllables have cognates in the "entering tone" syllables, which feature a -p, -t, -k in Middle Chinese and Southern Chinese varieties. The departing tone was analysed to feature a post-coda sibilant, "s". Clusters of -ps, -ts, -ks, were then formed at the end of syllables. These clusters eventually collapsed into "-ts" or "-s", before disappearing altogether, leaving elements of diphthongisation in more modern varieties. Old Vietnamese also had a rich inventory of initial clusters, but these were slowly merged with plain initials during Middle Vietnamese, and some have developed into the palatal nasal.
Origin
Some consonant clusters originate from the loss of a vowel in between two consonants, usually (but not always) due to vowel reduction caused by lack of stress. This is also the origin of most consonant clusters in English, some of which go back to Proto-Indo-European times. For example, ⟨glow⟩ comes from Proto-Germanic *glo-, which in turn comes from Proto-Indo-European *gʰel-ó, where *gʰel- is a root meaning 'to shine, to be bright' and is also present in ⟨glee⟩, ⟨gleam⟩, and ⟨glade⟩.
Consonant clusters can also originate from assimilation of a consonant with a vowel. In many Slavic languages, the combination mji, mje, mja etc. regularly gave mlji, mlje, mlja etc. Compare Russian zemlyá, which had this change, with Polish ziemia, which lacks the change, both from Proto-Balto-Slavic *źemē.[citation needed] See Proto-Slavic language and History of Proto-Slavic for more information about this change.
Clusters in languages
All languages differ in syllable structure and cluster template. A loanword from Adyghe in the extinct Ubykh language, psta ('to well up'), violates Ubykh's limit of two initial consonants. The English words sphere /ˈsfɪər/ and sphinx /ˈsfɪŋks/, Greek loanwords, break the rule that two fricatives may not appear adjacently word-initially. Some English words, including thrash, three, throat, and throw, start with the voiceless dental fricative /θ/, the liquid /r/, or the /r/ cluster (/θ/+/r/). This cluster example in Proto-Germanic has a counterpart in which /θ/ was followed by /l/. In early North and West Germanic, the /l/ cluster disappeared. This suggests that clusters are affected as words are loaned to other languages. The examples show that every language has syllable preference based on syllable structure and segment harmony of the language. Other factors that affect clusters when loaned to other languages include speech rate, articulatory factors, and speech perceptivity. Bayley has added that social factors such as age, gender, and geographical locations of speakers can determine clusters when they are loaned crosslinguistically.
English
In English, the longest possible initial cluster is three consonants, as in split /ˈsplɪt/, strudel /ˈstruːdəl/, strengths /ˈstrɛŋkθs/, and "squirrel" /ˈskwɪrəl/, all beginning with /s/ or /ʃ/, containing /p/, /t/, or /k/, and ending with /l/, /r/, or /w/; the longest possible final cluster is five consonants, as in angsts (/ˈæŋksts/),[citation needed] though this is rare (perhaps owing to being derived from a recent German loanword). However, the /k/ in angsts may also be considered epenthetic; for many speakers, nasal-sibilant sequences in the coda require insertion of a voiceless stop homorganic to the nasal. For speakers without this feature, the word is pronounced without the /k/. Final clusters of four consonants, as in angsts in other dialects (/ˈæŋsts/), twelfths /ˈtwɛlfθs/, sixths /ˈsɪksθs/, bursts /ˈbɜːrsts/ (in rhotic accents) and glimpsed /ˈɡlɪmpst/, are more common. Within compound words, clusters of five consonants or more are possible (if cross-syllabic clusters are accepted), as in handspring /ˈhændsprɪŋ/ and in the Yorkshire place-name of Hampsthwaite /hæmpsθweɪt/.[citation needed]
It is important to distinguish clusters and digraphs. Clusters are made of two or more consonant sounds, while a digraph is a group of two consonant letters standing for a single sound. For example, in the word ship, the two letters of the digraph ⟨sh⟩ together represent the single consonant [ʃ]. Conversely, the letter ⟨x⟩ can produce the consonant clusters /ks/ (annex), /gz/ (exist), /kʃ/ (sexual), or /gʒ/ (some pronunciations of "luxury"). It is worth noting that ⟨x⟩ often produces sounds in two different syllables (following the general principle of saturating the subsequent syllable before assigning sounds to the preceding syllable). Also note a combination digraph and cluster as seen in length with two digraphs ⟨ng⟩, ⟨th⟩ representing a cluster of two consonants: /ŋθ/ (although it may be pronounced /ŋkθ/ instead, as ⟨ng⟩ followed by a voiceless consonant in the same syllable often does); lights with a silent digraph ⟨gh⟩ followed by a cluster ⟨t⟩, ⟨s⟩: /ts/; and compound words such as sightscreen /ˈsaɪtskriːn/ or catchphrase /ˈkætʃfreɪz/.
Frequency
Not all consonant clusters are distributed equally among the languages of the world. Consonant clusters have a tendency to fall under patterns such as the sonority sequencing principle (SSP); the closer a consonant in a cluster is to the syllable's vowel, the more sonorous the consonant is. Among the most common types of clusters are initial stop-liquid sequences, such as in Thai (e.g. /pʰl/, /tr/, and /kl/). Other common ones include initial stop-approximant (e.g. Thai /kw/) and initial fricative-liquid (e.g. English /sl/) sequences. More rare are sequences which defy the SSP such as Proto-Indo-European /st/ and /spl/ (which many of its descendants have, including English). Certain consonants are more or less likely to appear in consonant clusters, especially in certain positions. The Tsou language of Taiwan has initial clusters such as /tf/, which doesn't violate the SSP, but nonetheless is unusual in having the labio-dental /f/ in the second position. The cluster /mx/ is also rare, but occurs in Russian words such as мха (/mxa/).
Consonant clusters at the ends of syllables are less common but follow the same principles. Clusters are more likely to begin with a liquid, approximant, or nasal and end with a fricative, affricate, or stop, such as in English "world" /wə(ɹ)ld/. Yet again, there are exceptions, such as English "lapse" /læps/.
See also
- English consonant cluster reductions
- Vowel cluster
- Conjunct consonant
- Consonant stacking
Notes
- If the ⟨ew⟩ /juː/ is thought of as consonant plus vowel rather than as a diphthong, three-consonant clusters also occur in words such as skew /ˈskjuː/
References
- "National reading panel, page 2-99" (PDF).
- "Phonics and Word Recognition Instruction in Early Reading Programs, Reading Rockets". 5 August 2013.
- J.C. Wells, Syllabification and allophony
- The extent of consonant clusters in Moroccan Arabic depends on the analysis. Richard Harrell's grammar of the language postulates schwa sounds in many positions that do not occur in other analyses. For example, the word that appears as ktbu "they wrote" in Jeffrey Heath's Ablaut and Ambiguity: Phonology of a Moroccan Arabic Dialect appears as ketbu in Harrell's grammar.
- Easterday, S. (2019). Highly Complex Syllable Structure: A Typological and Diachronic study (PDF). Berlin: Language Science Press. ISBN 9783961101955. Retrieved 30 July 2022.[page needed]
- Hank F. Nater (1984), The Bella Coola Language, Mercury Series, Canadian Ethnology Service (No. 92) (Ottawa: National Museums of Canada), cited in Bruce Bagemihl (1991), "Syllable Structure in Bella Coola", in the Proceedings of the New England Linguistics Society 21: 16–30
- Beebe, Leslie M. (1975). Occupational Prestige and Consonant Cluster Simplification in Bangkok Thai (PDF). International Journal of the Sociology of Language. Retrieved 23 January 2023.[page needed]
- Polgárdi, Krisztina (2015). "Syncope, syllabic consonant formation, and the distribution of stressed vowels in English". Journal of Linguistics. 51 (2): 383–423. doi:10.1017/S0022226714000486.
- Dziubalska-Kołaczyk, K. (2019). "On the structure, survival and change of consonant clusters". Folia Linguistica. 53(s40-s1) (53): 107. doi:10.1515/flih-2019-0006. S2CID 199546117.
- Côté, M. (2004). "Consonant cluster simplification in Québec French". Probus. 16 (2): 151. doi:10.1515/prbs.2004.16.2.151. S2CID 170972749.
- Guy, G. R.; Boyd, S. (1990). "The development of a morphological class". Language Variation and Change. 2 (1): 1. doi:10.1017/S0954394500000235. S2CID 145289008.
- Wilson, C. (2001). "Consonant cluster neutralisation and targeted constraints". Phonology. 18 (1): 147. doi:10.1017/S0952675701004043. S2CID 62574684.
- Jun, J. (2011). "Positional effects in consonant clusters". The Blackwell Companion to Phonology. 2: 1103.
- Bayley, R. (1994). "Consonant cluster reduction in Tejano English". Language Variation and Change. 6 (3): 303. doi:10.1017/S0954394500001708. S2CID 145407662.
- Harper, Douglas. "angst". Online Etymology Dictionary. Retrieved 17 March 2016.
This article has multiple issues Please help improve it or discuss these issues on the talk page Learn how and when to remove these messages This article needs additional citations for verification Please help improve this article by adding citations to reliable sources Unsourced material may be challenged and removed Find sources Consonant cluster news newspapers books scholar JSTOR April 2008 Learn how and when to remove this message This article possibly contains original research Please improve it by verifying the claims made and adding inline citations Statements consisting only of original research should be removed July 2022 Learn how and when to remove this message Learn how and when to remove this message This article contains phonetic transcriptions in the International Phonetic Alphabet IPA For an introductory guide on IPA symbols see Help IPA For the distinction between and see IPA Brackets and transcription delimiters In linguistics a consonant cluster consonant sequence or consonant compound is a group of consonants which have no intervening vowel In English for example the groups spl and ts are consonant clusters in the word splits In the education field it is variously called a consonant cluster or a consonant blend Some linguists who argue that the term can be properly applied only to those consonant clusters that occur within one syllable Others claim that the concept is more useful when it includes consonant sequences across syllable boundaries According to the former definition the longest consonant clusters in the word extra would be ks and tr whereas the latter allows kstr which is phonetically kst ɹ ʷ in some accents PhonotacticsThis section has multiple issues Please help improve it or discuss these issues on the talk page Learn how and when to remove these messages This section is written like an original unattributed academic essay that states a Wikipedia editor s personal feelings or presents an original argument about a topic Please help improve it by rewriting it in an encyclopedic style July 2022 Learn how and when to remove this message This section does not cite any sources Please help improve this section by adding citations to reliable sources Unsourced material may be challenged and removed July 2022 Learn how and when to remove this message Learn how and when to remove this message Each language has an associated set of phonotactic constraints Languages phonotactics differ as to what consonant clusters they permit Many languages are more restrictive than English in terms of consonant clusters and some forbid consonant clusters entirely For example Hawaiian like most Oceanic languages forbids consonant clusters entirely Japanese is almost as strict but allows a sequence of a nasal consonant plus another consonant as in Honshu hoꜜɰ ɕɯː the name of the largest island of Japan It also permits geminate kk pp ss and tt However palatalized consonants such as kʲ in Tōkyō toːkʲoː are single consonants Standard Arabic forbids initial consonant clusters and more than two consecutive consonants in other positions as do most other Semitic languages although Modern Israeli Hebrew permits initial two consonant clusters e g pkak cap dlaat pumpkin and Moroccan Arabic under Berber influence allows strings of several consonants Like most Mon Khmer languages Khmer permits only initial consonant clusters with up to three consonants in a row per syllable Finnish has initial consonant clusters natively only on South Western dialects and on foreign loans and only clusters of three inside the word are allowed Most spoken languages and dialects however are more permissive In Burmese consonant clusters of only up to three consonants the initial and two medials two written forms of j w at the initial onset are allowed in writing and only two the initial and one medial are pronounced these clusters are restricted to certain letters Some Burmese dialects allow for clusters of up to four consonants with the addition of the l medial which can combine with the above mentioned medials At the other end of the scale the Kartvelian languages of Georgia are drastically more permissive of consonant clustering Clusters in Georgian of four five or six consonants are not unusual for instance brtʼqʼɛli flat mt sʼvrtnɛli trainer and prt skvna peeling and if grammatical affixes are used it allows an eight consonant cluster ɡvbrdɣvnis he s plucking us gvprt skvni you peel us Consonants cannot appear as syllable nuclei in Georgian so this syllable is analysed as CCCCCCCCVC Many Slavic languages may manifest almost as formidable numbers of consecutive consonants such as in the Czech tongue twister Strc prst skrz krk pronounced str tʃ pr st skr s kr k meaning stick a finger through the neck the Slovak words stvrt ʃtvr c quarter and zblnknutie ʒbl ŋknucɪɛ clunk flop and the Slovene word skrbstvo skrbstʋo welfare However the liquid consonants r and l can form syllable nuclei in West and South Slavic languages and behave phonologically as vowels in this case An example of a true initial cluster is the Polish word wszczniesz fʂt ʂɲɛʂ you will initiate In the Serbo Croatian word opskrbljivanje ɔpskr bʎiʋaɲɛ victualling the lj and nj are digraphs representing single consonants ʎ and ɲ respectively In Dutch clusters of six or even seven consonants are possible e g angstschreeuw a scream of fear slechtstschrijvend writing the worst and zachtstschrijdend treading the most softly Some Salishan languages exhibit long words with no vowels at all such as the Nuxalk word xɬpʼxʷɬtʰɬpʰɬːskʷʰt sʼ he had had in his possession a bunchberry plant It is extremely difficult to accurately classify which of these consonants may be acting as the syllable nucleus and these languages challenge classical notions of exactly what constitutes a syllable The same problem is encountered in the Northern Berber languages There has been a trend to reduce and simplify consonant clusters in the Mainland Southeast Asia linguistic area such as Chinese and Vietnamese Old Chinese was known to contain additional medials such as r and or l which yielded retroflexion in Middle Chinese and today s Mandarin Chinese The word 江 read tɕiɑŋ in Mandarin and kɔːŋ in Cantonese is reconstructed as klong or krung in Old Chinese by Sinologists like Zhengzhang Shangfang William H Baxter and Laurent Sagart Additionally initial clusters such as tk and sn were analysed in recent reconstructions of Old Chinese and some were developed as palatalised sibilants Similarly in Thai words with initial consonant clusters are commonly reduced in colloquial speech to pronounce only the initial consonant such as the pronunciation of the word khrb reducing from kʰrap to kʰap Another element of consonant clusters in Old Chinese was analysed in coda and post coda position Some departing tone syllables have cognates in the entering tone syllables which feature a p t k in Middle Chinese and Southern Chinese varieties The departing tone was analysed to feature a post coda sibilant s Clusters of ps ts ks were then formed at the end of syllables These clusters eventually collapsed into ts or s before disappearing altogether leaving elements of diphthongisation in more modern varieties Old Vietnamese also had a rich inventory of initial clusters but these were slowly merged with plain initials during Middle Vietnamese and some have developed into the palatal nasal OriginSome consonant clusters originate from the loss of a vowel in between two consonants usually but not always due to vowel reduction caused by lack of stress This is also the origin of most consonant clusters in English some of which go back to Proto Indo European times For example glow comes from Proto Germanic glo which in turn comes from Proto Indo European gʰel o where gʰel is a root meaning to shine to be bright and is also present in glee gleam and glade Consonant clusters can also originate from assimilation of a consonant with a vowel In many Slavic languages the combination mji mje mja etc regularly gave mlji mlje mlja etc Compare Russian zemlya which had this change with Polish ziemia which lacks the change both from Proto Balto Slavic zeme citation needed See Proto Slavic language and History of Proto Slavic for more information about this change Clusters in languagesAll languages differ in syllable structure and cluster template A loanword from Adyghe in the extinct Ubykh language psta to well up violates Ubykh s limit of two initial consonants The English words sphere ˈsfɪer and sphinx ˈsfɪŋks Greek loanwords break the rule that two fricatives may not appear adjacently word initially Some English words including thrash three throat and throw start with the voiceless dental fricative 8 the liquid r or the r cluster 8 r This cluster example in Proto Germanic has a counterpart in which 8 was followed by l In early North and West Germanic the l cluster disappeared This suggests that clusters are affected as words are loaned to other languages The examples show that every language has syllable preference based on syllable structure and segment harmony of the language Other factors that affect clusters when loaned to other languages include speech rate articulatory factors and speech perceptivity Bayley has added that social factors such as age gender and geographical locations of speakers can determine clusters when they are loaned crosslinguistically EnglishIn English the longest possible initial cluster is three consonants as in split ˈsplɪt strudel ˈstruːdel strengths ˈstrɛŋk8s and squirrel ˈskwɪrel all beginning with s or ʃ containing p t or k and ending with l r or w the longest possible final cluster is five consonants as in angsts ˈaeŋksts citation needed though this is rare perhaps owing to being derived from a recent German loanword However the k in angsts may also be considered epenthetic for many speakers nasal sibilant sequences in the coda require insertion of a voiceless stop homorganic to the nasal For speakers without this feature the word is pronounced without the k Final clusters of four consonants as in angsts in other dialects ˈaeŋsts twelfths ˈtwɛlf8s sixths ˈsɪks8s bursts ˈbɜːrsts in rhotic accents and glimpsed ˈɡlɪmpst are more common Within compound words clusters of five consonants or more are possible if cross syllabic clusters are accepted as in handspring ˈhaendsprɪŋ and in the Yorkshire place name of Hampsthwaite haemps8weɪt citation needed It is important to distinguish clusters and digraphs Clusters are made of two or more consonant sounds while a digraph is a group of two consonant letters standing for a single sound For example in the word ship the two letters of the digraph sh together represent the single consonant ʃ Conversely the letter x can produce the consonant clusters ks annex gz exist kʃ sexual or gʒ some pronunciations of luxury It is worth noting that x often produces sounds in two different syllables following the general principle of saturating the subsequent syllable before assigning sounds to the preceding syllable Also note a combination digraph and cluster as seen in length with two digraphs ng th representing a cluster of two consonants ŋ8 although it may be pronounced ŋk8 instead as ng followed by a voiceless consonant in the same syllable often does lights with a silent digraph gh followed by a cluster t s ts and compound words such as sightscreen ˈsaɪtskriːn or catchphrase ˈkaetʃfreɪz FrequencyNot all consonant clusters are distributed equally among the languages of the world Consonant clusters have a tendency to fall under patterns such as the sonority sequencing principle SSP the closer a consonant in a cluster is to the syllable s vowel the more sonorous the consonant is Among the most common types of clusters are initial stop liquid sequences such as in Thai e g pʰl tr and kl Other common ones include initial stop approximant e g Thai kw and initial fricative liquid e g English sl sequences More rare are sequences which defy the SSP such as Proto Indo European st and spl which many of its descendants have including English Certain consonants are more or less likely to appear in consonant clusters especially in certain positions The Tsou language of Taiwan has initial clusters such as tf which doesn t violate the SSP but nonetheless is unusual in having the labio dental f in the second position The cluster mx is also rare but occurs in Russian words such as mha mxa Consonant clusters at the ends of syllables are less common but follow the same principles Clusters are more likely to begin with a liquid approximant or nasal and end with a fricative affricate or stop such as in English world we ɹ ld Yet again there are exceptions such as English lapse laeps See alsoEnglish consonant cluster reductions Vowel cluster Conjunct consonant Consonant stackingNotesIf the ew juː is thought of as consonant plus vowel rather than as a diphthong three consonant clusters also occur in words such as skew ˈskjuː References National reading panel page 2 99 PDF Phonics and Word Recognition Instruction in Early Reading Programs Reading Rockets 5 August 2013 J C Wells Syllabification and allophony The extent of consonant clusters in Moroccan Arabic depends on the analysis Richard Harrell s grammar of the language postulates schwa sounds in many positions that do not occur in other analyses For example the word that appears as ktbu they wrote in Jeffrey Heath s Ablaut and Ambiguity Phonology of a Moroccan Arabic Dialect appears as ketbu in Harrell s grammar Easterday S 2019 Highly Complex Syllable Structure A Typological and Diachronic study PDF Berlin Language Science Press ISBN 9783961101955 Retrieved 30 July 2022 page needed Hank F Nater 1984 The Bella Coola Language Mercury Series Canadian Ethnology Service No 92 Ottawa National Museums of Canada cited in Bruce Bagemihl 1991 Syllable Structure in Bella Coola in the Proceedings of the New England Linguistics Society 21 16 30 Beebe Leslie M 1975 Occupational Prestige and Consonant Cluster Simplification in Bangkok Thai PDF International Journal of the Sociology of Language Retrieved 23 January 2023 page needed Polgardi Krisztina 2015 Syncope syllabic consonant formation and the distribution of stressed vowels in English Journal of Linguistics 51 2 383 423 doi 10 1017 S0022226714000486 Dziubalska Kolaczyk K 2019 On the structure survival and change of consonant clusters Folia Linguistica 53 s40 s1 53 107 doi 10 1515 flih 2019 0006 S2CID 199546117 Cote M 2004 Consonant cluster simplification in Quebec French Probus 16 2 151 doi 10 1515 prbs 2004 16 2 151 S2CID 170972749 Guy G R Boyd S 1990 The development of a morphological class Language Variation and Change 2 1 1 doi 10 1017 S0954394500000235 S2CID 145289008 Wilson C 2001 Consonant cluster neutralisation and targeted constraints Phonology 18 1 147 doi 10 1017 S0952675701004043 S2CID 62574684 Jun J 2011 Positional effects in consonant clusters The Blackwell Companion to Phonology 2 1103 Bayley R 1994 Consonant cluster reduction in Tejano English Language Variation and Change 6 3 303 doi 10 1017 S0954394500001708 S2CID 145407662 Harper Douglas angst Online Etymology Dictionary Retrieved 17 March 2016