Blindsight is the ability of people who are cortically blind to respond to visual stimuli that they do not consciously see due to lesions in the primary visual cortex, also known as the striate cortex or Brodmann Area 17. The term was coined by Lawrence Weiskrantz and his colleagues in a paper published in a 1974 issue of Brain. A previous paper studying the discriminatory capacity of a cortically blind patient was published in Nature in 1973. The assumed existence of blindsight is controversial, with some arguing that it is merely degraded conscious vision.
Type classification
The majority of studies on blindsight are conducted on patients who are hemianopic, i.e. blind in one-half of their visual field. Following the destruction of the left or right striate cortex, patients are asked to detect, localize, and discriminate amongst visual stimuli that are presented to their blind side, often in a forced-response or guessing situation, even though they may not consciously recognize the visual stimulus. Research shows that such blind patients may achieve a higher accuracy than would be expected from chance alone.[citation needed]
Type 1 blindsight is the term given to this ability to guess—at levels significantly above chance—aspects of a visual stimulus (such as location or type of movement) without any conscious awareness of any stimuli. Type 2 blindsight occurs when patients claim to have a feeling that there has been a change within their blind area—e.g. movement—but that it was not a visual percept. The re-classification of blindsight into Type 1 and Type 2 was made after it was shown that the most celebrated blindsight patient, "GY", was usually conscious of stimuli presented to his blind field if the stimuli had certain specific characteristics, namely being of high contrast and moving fast (at speeds in excess of 20 degrees per second).
In the aftermath of the First World War, a neurologist, George Riddoch, had described patients who had been blinded by gunshot wounds to V1, who could not see stationary objects but who were, as he reported, "conscious" of seeing moving objects in their blind field. It is for this reason that the phenomenon has more recently also been called the Riddoch syndrome.
Since then it has become apparent that such subjects can also become aware of visual stimuli belonging to other visual domains, such as color and luminance, when presented to their blind fields. The ability of such hemianopic subjects to become consciously aware of stimuli presented to their blind field is also commonly referred to as "residual" or "degraded" vision.
As originally defined, blindsight challenged the common belief that perceptions must enter consciousness to affect our behavior, by showing that our behavior can be guided by sensory information of which we have no conscious awareness. Since the demonstration that blind patients can experience some visual stimuli consciously, and the consequent redefinition of blindsight into Type 1 and Type 2, a more nuanced view of the phenomenon has developed. Blindsight may be thought of as a converse of the form of anosognosia known as Anton syndrome, in which there is full cortical blindness along with the confabulation of visual experience.
History
Much of our current understanding of blindsight can be attributed to early experiments on monkeys. One monkey, named Helen, could be considered the "star monkey in visual research" because she was the original blindsight subject. Helen was a macaque monkey that had been decorticated; specifically, her primary visual cortex (V1) was completely removed, blinding her. Nevertheless, under certain specific situations, Helen exhibited sighted behavior. Her pupils would dilate and she would blink at stimuli that threatened her eyes. Furthermore, under certain experimental conditions, she could detect a variety of visual stimuli, such as the presence and location of objects, as well as shape, pattern, orientation, motion, and color.[page needed] In many cases, she was able to navigate her environment and interact with objects as if she were sighted.
A similar phenomenon was also discovered in humans. Subjects who had suffered damage to their visual cortices due to accidents or strokes reported partial or total blindness. Despite this, when prompted they could "guess" the presence and details of objects with above-average accuracy and, much like animal subjects, could catch objects tossed at them. The subjects never developed any kind of confidence in their abilities. Even when told of their successes, they would not begin to spontaneously make "guesses" about objects, but instead still required prompting. Furthermore, blindsight subjects rarely express the amazement about their abilities that sighted people would expect them to express.
Describing blindsight
Patients with blindsight have damage to the system that produces visual perception (the visual cortex of the brain and some of the nerve fibers that bring information to it from the eyes) rather than to the underlying brain system controlling eye movements. The phenomenon was originally thought to show how, after the more complex perception system is damaged, people can use the underlying control system to guide hand movements towards an object even though they cannot see what they are reaching for. Hence, visual information can control behavior without producing a conscious sensation. This ability of those with blindsight to act as if able to see objects that they are unconscious of suggested that consciousness is not a general property of all parts of the brain, but is produced by specialized parts of it.
Blindsight patients show awareness of single visual features, such as edges and motion, but cannot gain a holistic visual percept. This suggests that perceptual awareness is modular and that—in sighted individuals—there is a "binding process that unifies all information into a whole percept", which is interrupted in patients with such conditions as blindsight and visual agnosia. Therefore, object identification and object recognition are thought to be separate processes and occur in different areas of the brain, working independently from one another. The modular theory of object perception and integration would account for the "hidden perception" experienced in blindsight patients. Research has shown that visual stimuli with the single visual features of sharp borders, sharp onset/offset times, motion and low spatial frequency contribute to, but are not strictly necessary for, an object's salience in blindsight.
Cause
There are three theories for the explanation of blindsight. The first states that after damage to area V1, other branches of the optic nerve deliver visual information to the superior colliculus, pulvinar and several other areas, including parts of the cerebral cortex. In turn, these areas might then control the blindsight responses.
Another explanation for the phenomenon of blindsight is that even though the majority of a person's visual cortex may be damaged, tiny islands of functioning tissue remain. These islands are not large enough to provide conscious perception, but nevertheless enough for some unconscious visual perception.
A third theory is that the information required to determine the distance to and velocity of an object in object space is determined by the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) before the information is projected to the visual cortex. In a normal subject, these signals are used to merge the information from the eyes into a three-dimensional representation (which includes the position and velocity of individual objects relative to the organism), extract a vergence signal to benefit the precision (previously auxiliary) optical system, and extract a focus control signal for the lenses of the eyes. The stereoscopic information is attached to the object information passed to the visual cortex.
More recently, with the demonstration of a direct input from the LGN to area V5 (MT), which delivers signals from fast moving stimuli at latencies of about 30 ms, another explanation has emerged. This one proposes that the delivery of these signals is sufficient to arouse a conscious experience of fast visual motion, without implying that it is V5 alone that is responsible, since once signals reach V5, they may be propagated to other areas of the brain. The latter account would seem to exclude the possibility that signals are "pre-processed" by V1 or "post-processed" by it (through return connections from V5 back to V1), as has been suggested. The pulvinar nucleus of the thalamus also sends direct, V1 by-passing, signals to V5 but their precise role in generating a conscious visual experience of motion has not yet been determined.
Evidence of blindsight can be indirectly observed in children as young as two months, although there is difficulty in determining the type in a patient who is not old enough to answer questions.
Evidence in animals
In a 1995 experiment, researchers attempted to show that monkeys with lesions in or even wholly removed striate cortexes also experienced blindsight. To study this, they had the monkeys complete tasks similar to those commonly used for human subjects. The monkeys were placed in front of a monitor and taught to indicate whether a stationary object or nothing was present in their visual field when a tone was played. Then the monkeys performed the same task except the stationary objects were presented outside of their visual field. The monkeys performed very similar to human participants and were unable to perceive the presence of stationary objects outside of their visual field.
Another 1995 study by the same group sought to prove that monkeys could also be conscious of movement in their deficit visual field despite not being consciously aware of the presence of an object there. To do this, researchers used another standard test for humans which was similar to the previous study except moving objects were presented in the deficit visual field. Starting from the center of the deficit visual field, the object would either move up, down, or to the right. The monkeys performed identically to humans on the test, getting them right almost every time. This showed that the monkey's ability to detect movement is separate from their ability to consciously detect an object in their deficit visual field, and gave further evidence for the claim that damage to the striate cortex plays a large role in causing the disorder.
Several years later, another study compared and contrasted the data collected from monkeys and that of a specific human patient with blindsight, GY. GY's striate cortical region was damaged through trauma at the age of eight, though for the most part he retained full functionality, GY was not consciously aware of anything in his right visual field. In the monkeys, the striate cortex of the left hemisphere was surgically removed. By comparing the test results of both GY and the monkeys, the researchers concluded that similar patterns of responses to stimuli in the "blind" visual field can be found in both species.
Research
Lawrence Weiskrantz and colleagues showed in the early 1970s that if forced to guess about whether a stimulus is present in their blind field, some observers do better than chance.[page needed] This ability to detect stimuli that the observer is not conscious of can extend to discrimination of the type of stimulus (for example, whether an 'X' or 'O' has been presented in the blind field).[citation needed]
Electrophysiological evidence from the late 1970s has shown that there is no direct retinal input from S-cones to the superior colliculus, implying that the perception of color information should be impaired. However, more recent evidence point to a pathway from S-cones to the superior colliculus, opposing previous research and supporting the idea that some chromatic processing mechanisms are intact in blindsight.
Patients shown images on their blind side of people expressing emotions correctly guessed the emotion most of the time. The movement of facial muscles used in smiling and frowning were measured and reacted in ways that matched the kind of emotion in the unseen image. Therefore, the emotions were recognized without involving conscious sight.
A 2011 study found that a young woman with a unilateral lesion of area V1 could scale her grasping movement as she reached out to pick up objects of different sizes placed in her blind field, even though she could not report the sizes of the objects. Similarly, another patient with unilateral lesion of area V1 could avoid obstacles placed in his blind field when he reached toward a target that was visible in his intact visual field. Even though he avoided the obstacles, he never reported seeing them.
A study reported in 2008 asked patient GY to misstate where in his visual field a distinctive stimulus was presented. If the stimulus was in the upper part of his visual field, he was to say it was in the lower part, and vice versa. He was able to misstate, as requested, in his left visual field (with normal conscious vision); but he tended to fail in the task—to state the location correctly—when the stimulus was in his blindsight (right) visual field. This failure rate worsened when the stimulus was clearer, indicating that failure was not simply due to unreliability of blindsight.
Case studies
This section's tone or style may not reflect the encyclopedic tone used on Wikipedia.(January 2018) |
"DB"
Researchers applied the same type of tests that were used to study blindsight in animals to a patient referred to as "DB". The normal techniques used to assess visual acuity in humans involved asking them to verbally describe some visually recognizable aspect of an object or objects. DB was given forced-choice tasks to complete instead. The results of DB's guesses showed that DB was able to determine shape and detect movement at some unconscious level, despite not being visually aware of this. DB himself chalked up the accuracy of his guesses to be merely coincidental.
The discovery of the condition known as blindsight raised questions about how different types of visual information, even unconscious information, may be affected and sometimes even unaffected by damage to different areas of the visual cortex. Previous studies had already demonstrated that even without conscious awareness of visual stimuli, humans could still determine certain visual features such as presence in the visual field, shape, orientation and movement. But, in a newer study evidence showed that if damage to the visual cortex occurs in areas above the primary visual cortex, the conscious awareness of visual stimuli itself is not damaged. Blindsight shows that even when the primary visual cortex is damaged or removed a person can still perform actions guided by unconscious visual information. Despite damage occurring in the area necessary for conscious awareness of visual information, other functions of the processing of these visual percepts are still available to the individual. The same also goes for damage to other areas of the visual cortex. If an area of the cortex that is responsible for a certain function is damaged, it will only result in the loss of that particular function or aspect, functions that other parts of the visual cortex are responsible for remain intact.
Alexander and Cowey
Alexander and Cowey investigated how contrasting stimuli brightness affects blindsight patients' ability to discern movement. Prior studies have already shown that blindsight patients are able to detect motion even though they claim they do not see any visual percepts in their blind fields. The study subjects were two patients who suffered from hemianopsia—blindness in more than half of their visual field. Both subjects had displayed the ability to accurately determine the presence of visual stimuli in their blind hemifields without acknowledging an actual visual percept previously.
To test the effect of brightness on the subject's ability to determine motion they used a white background with a series of colored dots. The contrast of the brightness of the dots compared to the white background was altered in each trial to determine if the participants performed better or worse when there was a larger discrepancy in brightness or not. The subjects focused on the display for two equal length time intervals and were asked whether they thought the dots were moving during the first or the second time interval.
When the contrast in brightness between the background and the dots was higher, both of the subjects could discern motion more accurately than they would have statistically through guesswork. However, one subject was not able to accurately determine whether or not blue dots were moving regardless of the brightness contrast, but he/she was able to do so with every other color dot. When the contrast was highest, subjects were able to tell whether or not the dots were moving with very high rates of accuracy. Even when the dots were white, but still of a different brightness from the background, subjects could still determine whether they were moving. But, regardless of the dots' color, subjects could not tell when they were in motion when the white background and the dots were of similar brightness.
Kentridge, Heywood, and Weiskrantz
Kentridge, Heywood, and Weiskrantz used the phenomenon of blindsight to investigate the connection between visual attention and visual awareness. They wanted to see if their subject—who exhibited blindsight in other studies—could react more quickly when their attention was cued without the ability to be visually aware of it. The researchers aimed to show that being conscious of a stimulus and paying attention to it was not the same thing.
To test the relationship between attention and awareness, they had the participant try to determine where a target was and whether it was oriented horizontally or vertically on a computer screen. The target line would appear at one of two different locations and would be oriented in one of two directions. Before the target would appear an arrow would become visible on the screen, sometimes pointing to the correct position of the target line and less frequently not. This arrow was the cue for the subject. The participant would press a key to indicate whether the line was horizontal or vertical, and could then also indicate to an observer whether or not he/she actually had a feeling that any object was there or not—even if they couldn't see anything. The participant was able to accurately determine the orientation of the line when the target was cued by an arrow before the appearance of the target, even though these visual stimuli did not equal awareness in the subject who had no vision in that area of his/her visual field. The study showed that even without the ability to be visually aware of a stimulus the participant could still focus his/her attention on this object.
"TN"
In 2003, a patient known as "TN" lost use of his primary visual cortex, area V1. He had two successive strokes, which knocked out the region in both his left and right hemispheres. After his strokes, ordinary tests of TN's sight turned up nothing. He could not even detect large objects moving right in front of his eyes. Researchers eventually began to notice that TN exhibited signs of blindsight and in 2008 decided to test their theory. They took TN into a hallway and asked him to walk through it without using the cane he always carried after having the strokes. TN was not aware at the time, but the researchers had placed various obstacles in the hallway to test if he could avoid them without conscious use of his sight. To the researchers' delight, he moved around every obstacle with ease, at one point even pressing himself up against the wall to squeeze past a trashcan placed in his way. After navigating through the hallway, TN reported that he was just walking the way he wanted to, not because he knew anything was there.
"Mr. J"
In another case study, a girl brought her grandfather in to see a neuropsychologist. The girl's grandfather, Mr. J., had suffered a stroke that had left him completely blind apart from a tiny spot in the middle of his visual field. The neuropsychologist, Dr. M., performed an exercise with him. The doctor helped Mr. J. to a chair, had him sit down, and then asked to borrow his cane. The doctor then asked, "Mr. J., please look straight ahead. Keep looking that way, and don't move your eyes or turn your head. I know that you can see a little bit straight ahead of you, and I don't want you to use that piece of vision for what I'm going to ask you to do. Fine. Now, I'd like you to reach out with your right hand [and] point to what I'm holding." Mr. J. then replied, "But I don't see anything—I'm blind!" The doctor then said, "I know, but please try, anyway." Mr. J then shrugged and pointed, and was surprised when his finger encountered the end of the cane which the doctor was pointing toward him. After this, Mr. J. said that "it was just luck". The doctor then turned the cane around so that the handle side was pointing towards Mr. J. He then asked for Mr. J. to grab hold of the cane. Mr. J. reached out with an open hand and grabbed hold of the cane. After this, the doctor said, "Good. Now put your hand down, please." The doctor then rotated the cane 90 degrees, so that the handle was oriented vertically. The doctor then asked Mr. J. to reach for the cane again. Mr. J. did this, turning his wrist so that his hand matched the orientation of the handle. This case study shows that, although (on a conscious level) Mr. J. was completely unaware of any visual abilities that he may have had, he was able to orient his grabbing motions as if he had no visual impairments.
Other cases
Other cases refer to SL, GY and GR.
Brain regions involved
This section needs more reliable medical references for verification or relies too heavily on primary sources, specifically: only final sentence appears to be inline referenced.(August 2015) |
Visual processing in the brain goes through a series of stages. Destruction of the primary visual cortex leads to blindness in the part of the visual field that corresponds to the damaged cortical representation. The area of blindness – known as a scotoma – is in the visual field opposite the damaged hemisphere and can vary from a small area up to the entire hemifield. Visual processing occurs in the brain in a hierarchical series of stages (with much crosstalk and feedback between areas). The route from the retina through V1 is not the only visual pathway into the cortex, though it is by far the largest; it is commonly thought that the residual performance of people exhibiting blindsight is due to preserved pathways into the extrastriate cortex that bypass V1. However both physiological evidence in monkeys and behavioral and imaging evidence in humans shows that activity in these extrastriate areas, and especially in V5, is apparently sufficient to support visual awareness in the absence of V1.
To put it in a more complex way, recent physiological findings suggest that visual processing takes place along several independent, parallel pathways. One system processes information about shape, one about color, and one about movement, location and spatial organization. This information moves through an area of the brain called the lateral geniculate nucleus, located in the thalamus, and on to be processed in the primary visual cortex, area V1 (also known as the striate cortex because of its striped appearance). People with damage to V1 report no conscious vision, no visual imagery, and no visual images in their dreams. However, some of these people still experience the blindsight phenomenon, though this too is controversial, with some studies showing a limited amount of consciousness without V1 or projections relating to it.
The superior colliculus and prefrontal cortex also have a major role in awareness of a visual stimulus.
Lateral geniculate nucleus
Mosby's Dictionary of Medicine, Nursing & Health Professions defines the LGN as "one of two elevations of the lateral posterior thalamus receiving visual impulses from the retina via the optic nerves and tracts and relaying the impulses to the calcarine (visual) cortex".
What is seen in the left and right visual field is taken in by each eye and brought back to the optic disc via the nerve fibres of the retina. From the optic disc, visual information travels through the optic nerve and into the optic chiasm. Visual information then enters the optic tract and travels to four different areas of the brain including the superior colliculus, pretectum of the mid brain, the suprachiasmatic nucleus of the hypothalamus, and the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN). Most axons from the LGN will then travel to the primary visual cortex.
Injury to the primary visual cortex, including lesions and other trauma, leads to the loss of visual experience. However, the residual vision that is left cannot be attributed to V1. According to Schmid et al., "thalamic lateral geniculate nucleus has a causal role in V1-independent processing of visual information". This information was found through experiments using fMRI during activation and inactivation of the LGN and the contribution the LGN has on visual experience in monkeys with a V1 lesion. These researchers concluded that the magnocellular system of the LGN is less affected by the removal of V1, which suggests that it is because of this system in the LGN that blindsight occurs. Furthermore, once the LGN was inactivated, virtually all of the extrastriate areas of the brain no longer showed a response on the fMRI. The information leads to a qualitative assessment that included "scotoma stimulation, with the LGN intact had fMRI activation of ~20% of that under normal conditions". This finding agrees with the information obtained from, and fMRI images of, patients with blindsight. The same study also supported the conclusion that the LGN plays a substantial role in blindsight. Specifically, while injury to V1 does create a loss of vision, the LGN is less affected and may result in the residual vision that remains, causing the "sight" in blindsight.
Functional magnetic resonance imaging has launched has also been employed to conduct brain scans in normal, healthy human volunteers to attempt to demonstrate that visual motion can bypass V1, through a connection from the LGN to the human middle temporal complex. Their findings concluded that there was an indeed a connection of visual motion information that went directly from the LGN to the V5/hMT+ bypassing V1 completely. Evidence also suggests that, following a traumatic injury to V1, there is still a direct pathway from the retina through the LGN to the extrastriate visual areas. The extrastriate visual areas include parts of the occipital lobe that surround V1. In non-human primates, these often include V2, V3, and V4.
In a study conducted in primates, after partial ablation of area V1, areas V2 and V3 were still excited by visual stimulus. Other evidence suggests that "the LGN projections that survive V1 removal are relatively sparse in density, but are nevertheless widespread and probably encompass all extrastriate visual areas," including V2, V4, V5 and the inferotemporal cortex region.
Controversy
The results of some experiments suggest that blindsighted people may be preserving some kind of conscious experience and thus they are not fully blind. The criteria for blindsight has repeatedly changed based on findings that challenge the original definition, which has led some scientists to cast doubt on the existence of blindsight.
See also
- Koniocellular cell
- Riddoch syndrome
- Two-streams hypothesis
- Visual agnosia
References
- Celesia G (2010). "Visual perception and awareness: a modular system". Journal of Psychophysiology. 24 (2): 62–67. doi:10.1027/0269-8803/a000014.
- Weiskrantz L, Warrington EK, Sanders MD, Marshall J (December 1974). "Visual capacity in the hemianopic field following a restricted occipital ablation". Brain. 97 (4): 709–28. doi:10.1093/brain/97.1.709. PMID 4434190.
- Poppel E, Held R, Frost D (June 1973). "Leter: Residual visual function after brain wounds involving the central visual pathways in man". Nature. 243 (5405): 295–6. doi:10.1038/243295a0. PMID 4774871. S2CID 4160116.
- Overgaard, Morten (November 2012). "Blindsight: recent and historical controversies on the blindness of blindsight". WIREs Cognitive Science. 3 (6): 607–614. doi:10.1002/wcs.1194. ISSN 1939-5078. PMID 26305269.
- Overgaard M, Fehl K, Mouridsen K, Bergholt B, Cleeremans A (August 2008). "Seeing without Seeing? Degraded Conscious Vision in a Blindsight Patient". PLOS ONE. 3 (8): e3028. Bibcode:2008PLoSO...3.3028O. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0003028. PMC 2507770. PMID 18716654.
- Mazzi C, Bagattini C, Savazzi S (2016). "Blind-Sight vs. Degraded-Sight: Different Measures Tell a Different Story". Frontiers in Psychology. 7: 901. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00901. PMC 4909743. PMID 27378993.
- Weiskrantz L (1997). Consciousness Lost and Found: A Neuropsychological Exploration. Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-852301-7.
- Barbur JL, Watson JD, Frackowiak RS, Zeki S (December 1993). "Conscious visual perception without V1". Brain. 116 ( Pt 6) (6): 1293–302. doi:10.1093/brain/116.6.1293. PMID 8293272.
- Stoerig, Petra; Barth, Erhardt (2001-12-01). "Low-Level Phenomenal Vision Despite Unilateral Destruction of Primary Visual Cortex". Consciousness and Cognition. 10 (4): 574–587. doi:10.1006/ccog.2001.0526. ISSN 1053-8100. PMID 11790045. S2CID 22895605. Archived from the original on 2021-04-24. Retrieved 2021-04-24.
- Riddoch G (1917-05-01). "Dissociation of Visual Perceptions Due to Occipital Injuries, With Especial Reference to Appreciation of movement". Brain. 40 (1): 15–57. doi:10.1093/brain/40.1.15. ISSN 0006-8950.
- Zeki S, Ffytche DH (January 1998). "The Riddoch syndrome: insights into the neurobiology of conscious vision". Brain. 121 ( Pt 1) (1): 25–45. doi:10.1093/brain/121.1.25. PMID 9549486.
- Morland AB, Jones SR, Finlay AL, Deyzac E, Lê S, Kemp S (June 1999). "Visual perception of motion, luminance and colour in a human hemianope". Brain. 122 ( Pt 6) (6): 1183–98. doi:10.1093/brain/122.6.1183. PMID 10356069.
- Carlson N (2013). Physiology of Behavior (11th ed.). University of Massachusetts, Amherst: Pearson Education, Inc. p. 4. ISBN 978-0-205-23981-8.
- Weisberg, Josh (June 2006). "Red in the head". Nature. 441 (7093): 577. Bibcode:2006Natur.441..577W. doi:10.1038/441577a. ISSN 1476-4687. S2CID 26642958.
- Humphrey NK (1970). "What the frog's eye tells the monkey's brain". Brain, Behavior and Evolution. 3 (1): 324–37. doi:10.1159/000125480. PMID 5001242.
- Humphrey NK (1974). "Vision in a monkey without striate cortex: a case study". Perception. 3 (3): 241–55. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.452.5493. doi:10.1068/p030241. PMID 4459818. S2CID 4686081.
- Humphrey N (1992). A History of the Mind. New York: Simon & Schuster. ISBN 9780671686444. OCLC 25915998.
- Holt J (2003). Blindsight and the Nature of Consciousness. Peterborough, Ontario: Broadview Press. ISBN 978-1-55111-351-7. OCLC 50755257.[page needed]
- Humphrey N (2006). Seeing Red: A Study in Consciousness. Mind/brain/behavior initiative. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Belknap Press. ISBN 978-0-674-02179-2. OCLC 234101445.[page needed]
- Alexander I, Cowey A (June 2010). "Edges, colour and awareness in blindsight". Consciousness and Cognition. 19 (2): 520–33. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2010.01.008. PMID 20171122. S2CID 36139700.
- Ffytche DH, Zeki S (January 2011). "The primary visual cortex, and feedback to it, are not necessary for conscious vision". Brain. 134 (Pt 1): 247–57. doi:10.1093/brain/awq305. PMC 3159156. PMID 21097490.
- Sahraie A, Hibbard PB, Trevethan CT, Ritchie KL, Weiskrantz L (December 2010). "Consciousness of the first order in blindsight". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 107 (49): 21217–22. Bibcode:2010PNAS..10721217S. doi:10.1073/pnas.1015652107. PMC 3000284. PMID 21078979.
- Kinoshita M, Kato R, Isa K, Kobayashi K, Kobayashi K, Onoe H, Isa T (2019-01-11). "Dissecting the circuit for blindsight to reveal the critical role of pulvinar and superior colliculus". Nat Commun. 10 (1): 135. Bibcode:2019NatCo..10..135K. doi:10.1038/s41467-018-08058-0. PMC 6329824. PMID 30635570. S2CID 58009143.
- Kletenik I, Ferguson MA, Bateman JR, Cohen AL, Lin C, Tetreault A, Pelak VS, Anderson CA, Prasad S, Darby RR, Fox MD (2021-12-27). "Network Localization of Unconscious Visual Perception in Blindsight". Ann Neurol. 91 (2): 217–224. doi:10.1002/ana.26292. PMC 10013845. PMID 34961965. S2CID 245553461.
- Radoeva PD, Prasad S, Brainard DH, Aguirre GK (2008-11-20). "Neural activity within area V1 reflects unconscious visual performance in a case of blindsight". J Cogn Neurosci. 20 (11): 1927–1939. doi:10.1162/jocn.2008.20139. PMC 2773243. PMID 18416678.
- Kalat JW (2009). Biological Psychology (10th ed.). Belmont, California: Wadsworth. pp. 169–170. ISBN 9780495603009. OCLC 236316740.
- Fulton, J. (2004) Processes in Biological Vision Section 7.4 "Archived copy" (PDF). Archived from the original on 2015-02-21. Retrieved 2012-11-26.
{{cite web}}
: CS1 maint: archived copy as title (link) - Benevento LA, Yoshida K (December 1981). "The afferent and efferent organization of the lateral geniculo-prestriate pathways in the macaque monkey". The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 203 (3): 455–74. doi:10.1002/cne.902030309. PMID 6274921. S2CID 28585691.
- Fries W (September 1981). "The projection from the lateral geniculate nucleus to the prestriate cortex of the macaque monkey". Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences. 213 (1190): 73–86. Bibcode:1981RSPSB.213...73F. doi:10.1098/rspb.1981.0054. PMID 6117869. S2CID 5700048.
- Yukie M, Iwai E (September 1981). "Direct projection from the dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus to the prestriate cortex in macaque monkeys". The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 201 (1): 81–97. doi:10.1002/cne.902010107. PMID 7276252. S2CID 8825689.
- Sincich LC, Park KF, Wohlgemuth MJ, Horton JC (October 2004). "Bypassing V1: a direct geniculate input to area MT". Nature Neuroscience. 7 (10): 1123–8. doi:10.1038/nn1318. PMID 15378066. S2CID 13419990.
- ffytche DH, Guy CN, Zeki S (December 1995). "The parallel visual motion inputs into areas V1 and V5 of human cerebral cortex". Brain. 118 ( Pt 6) (6): 1375–94. doi:10.1093/brain/118.6.1375. PMID 8595471.
- Beckers G, Zeki S (February 1995). "The consequences of inactivating areas V1 and V5 on visual motion perception". Brain. 118 ( Pt 1) (1): 49–60. doi:10.1093/brain/118.1.49. PMID 7895014.
- Schmid MC, Mrowka SW, Turchi J, Saunders RC, Wilke M, Peters AJ, et al. (July 2010). "Blindsight depends on the lateral geniculate nucleus". Nature. 466 (7304): 373–7. Bibcode:2010Natur.466..373S. doi:10.1038/nature09179. PMC 2904843. PMID 20574422.
- Ffytche DH, Zeki S (January 2011). "The primary visual cortex, and feedback to it, are not necessary for conscious vision". Brain. 134 (Pt 1): 247–57. doi:10.1093/brain/awq305. PMC 3159156. PMID 21097490.
- Lamme, VAF (2001-04-01). "Blindsight: the role of feedforward and feedback corticocortical connections". Acta Psychologica. 107 (1–3): 209–228. doi:10.1016/S0001-6918(01)00020-8. ISSN 0001-6918. PMID 11388136. Archived from the original on 2021-04-27. Retrieved 2021-04-09.
- Cragg, B.G. (1969-07-01). "The topography of the afferent projections in the circumstriate visual cortex of the monkey studied by the nauta method". Vision Research. 9 (7): 733–747. doi:10.1016/0042-6989(69)90011-X. ISSN 0042-6989. PMID 4979024. Archived from the original on 2022-08-13. Retrieved 2021-04-09.
- Boyle NJ, Jones DH, Hamilton R, Spowart KM, Dutton GN (October 2005). "Blindsight in children: does it exist and can it be used to help the child? Observations on a case series". Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology. 47 (10): 699–702. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8749.2005.tb01057.x. PMID 16174315.
- Cowey A, Stoerig P (January 1995). "Blindsight in monkeys" (PDF). Nature. 373 (6511): 247–9. Bibcode:1995Natur.373..247C. doi:10.1038/373247a0. PMID 7816139. S2CID 4269412. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2012-05-01. Retrieved 2018-02-05.
- Stoerig P, Cowey A (March 1997). "Blindsight in man and monkey". Brain. 120 ( Pt 3) (3): 535–59. doi:10.1093/brain/120.3.535. PMID 9126063.[dead link ]
- Cowey A, Alexander I, Stoerig P (February 2008). "A blindsight conundrum: how to respond when there isn't a correct response". Neuropsychologia. 46 (3): 870–8. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.11.031. PMID 18201733. S2CID 20790934.
- Weiskrantz L (1986). Blindsight: A Case Study and Implications. Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-852192-1. OCLC 21677307.
- de Monasterio FM (November 1978). "Properties of ganglion cells with atypical receptive-field organization in retina of macaques". Journal of Neurophysiology. 41 (6): 1435–49. doi:10.1152/jn.1978.41.6.1435. PMID 104014.
- Marrocco RT, Li RH (July 1977). "Monkey superior colliculus: properties of single cells and their afferent inputs". Journal of Neurophysiology. 40 (4): 844–60. doi:10.1152/jn.1977.40.4.844. PMID 407333.
- Schiller PH, Malpeli JG (March 1977). "Properties and tectal projections of monkey retinal ganglion cells". Journal of Neurophysiology. 40 (2): 428–45. doi:10.1152/jn.1977.40.2.428. PMID 403252.
- Hall NJ, Colby CL (2009). "Response to blue visual stimuli in the macaque superior colliculus". Society for Neuroscience. 19: 520–533.
- Garrett B (2011). Brain & Behavior: An Introduction to Biological Psychology (3rd ed.). Thousand Oaks, California: SAGE Publications. pp. 315–318. ISBN 978-1-4129-8168-2. OCLC 617425474.
- Tamietto M, de Gelder B (October 2010). "Neural bases of the non-conscious perception of emotional signals". Nature Reviews. Neuroscience. 11 (10): 697–709. doi:10.1038/nrn2889. hdl:2318/79483. PMID 20811475. S2CID 4690318.
- Whitwell RL, Striemer CL, Nicolle DA, Goodale MA (April 2011). "Grasping the non-conscious: preserved grip scaling to unseen objects for immediate but not delayed grasping following a unilateral lesion to primary visual cortex". Vision Research. 51 (8): 908–24. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2011.02.005. PMID 21324336.
- Striemer CL, Chapman CS, Goodale MA (September 2009). ""Real-time" obstacle avoidance in the absence of primary visual cortex". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 106 (37): 15996–6001. Bibcode:2009PNAS..10615996S. doi:10.1073/pnas.0905549106. PMC 2747232. PMID 19805240.
- Persaud N, Cowey A (September 2008). "Blindsight is unlike normal conscious vision: evidence from an exclusion task". Consciousness and Cognition. 17 (3): 1050–5. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2007.10.002. PMID 18065242. S2CID 30699219.
- Weiskrantz L (2007). "Blindsight". Scholarpedia. 2 (4): 3047. Bibcode:2007SchpJ...2.3047W. doi:10.4249/scholarpedia.3047.
- Stoerig P (September 1996). "Varieties of vision: from blind responses to conscious recognition". Trends in Neurosciences. 19 (9): 401–6. doi:10.1016/S0166-2236(96)10051-5. PMID 8873358. S2CID 25012895.
- Alexander I, Cowey A (March 2013). "Isoluminant coloured stimuli are undetectable in blindsight even when they move". Experimental Brain Research. 225 (1): 147–52. doi:10.1007/s00221-012-3355-6. PMID 23263562. S2CID 1738371.
- Kentridge RW, Heywood CA, Weiskrantz L (2004). "Spatial attention speeds discrimination without awareness in blindsight". Neuropsychologia. 42 (6): 831–5. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.719.7118. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.11.001. PMID 15037061. S2CID 12837840.
- de Gelder B, Tamietto M, van Boxtel G, Goebel R, Sahraie A, van den Stock J, et al. (December 2008). "Intact navigation skills after bilateral loss of striate cortex". Current Biology. 18 (24): R1128-9. Bibcode:2008CBio...18R1128D. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2008.11.002. PMID 19108766.
- Rodman HR, Gross CG, Albright TD (June 1989). "Afferent basis of visual response properties in area MT of the macaque. I. Effects of striate cortex removal". The Journal of Neuroscience. 9 (6): 2033–50. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.09-06-02033.1989. PMC 6569731. PMID 2723765.
- Gaglianese A, Costagli M, Bernardi G, Ricciardi E, Pietrini P (April 2012). "Evidence of a direct influence between the thalamus and hMT+ independent of V1 in the human brain as measured by fMRI". NeuroImage. 60 (2): 1440–7. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.093. PMID 22300813. S2CID 937762.
- Ffytche DH, Zeki S (January 2011). "The primary visual cortex, and feedback to it, are not necessary for conscious vision". Brain. 134 (Pt 1): 247–57. doi:10.1093/brain/awq305. PMC 3159156. PMID 21097490.
- Mosby's Dictionary of Medicine, Nursing & Health Professions. Mosby, Inc. (8th ed.). St. Louis, Missouri: Mosby/Elsevier. 2009. ISBN 9780323049375. OCLC 226911727.
{{cite book}}
: CS1 maint: others (link) - Dragoi V (1997). "Chapter 15: Visual Processing: Cortical Pathways". Neuroscience Online. The University of Texas Health Science Center at Houston. Archived from the original on July 26, 2024. Retrieved November 3, 2013.
- Cowey A (September 2010). "Visual system: how does blindsight arise?". Current Biology. 20 (17): R702-4. Bibcode:2010CBio...20.R702C. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2010.07.014. PMID 20833309. S2CID 17351599.
- Weiskrantz L (April 1996). "Blindsight revisited". Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 6 (2): 215–20. doi:10.1016/s0959-4388(96)80075-4. PMID 8725963. S2CID 1833570.
Further reading
- "Blindsight: How brain sees what you do not see". Medical Press. 14 October 2008. Archived from the original on 6 February 2018. Retrieved 5 February 2018.
- Collins GP. "Blindsight: Seeing without knowing it". Scientific American Blog Network. Archived from the original on 26 July 2024. Retrieved 5 February 2018.
- Danckert J, Rossetti Y (2005). "Blindsight in action: what can the different sub-types of blindsight tell us about the control of visually guided actions?". Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 29 (7): 1035–46. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.02.001. PMID 16143169. S2CID 12833434.
- De Gelder B (May 2010). "Uncanny sight in the blind". Scientific American. 302 (5): 60–5. Bibcode:2010SciAm.302e..60D. doi:10.1038/scientificamerican0510-60. PMID 20443379.
- Leh SE, Johansen-Berg H, Ptito A (July 2006). "Unconscious vision: new insights into the neuronal correlate of blindsight using diffusion tractography". Brain. 129 (Pt 7): 1822–32. doi:10.1093/brain/awl111. PMID 16714319.
- Leh SE, Mullen KT, Ptito A (November 2006). "Absence of S-cone input in human blindsight following hemispherectomy". The European Journal of Neuroscience. 24 (10): 2954–60. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.578.4900. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.05178.x. PMID 17156217. S2CID 14152585.
- McIntosh AR, Rajah MN, Lobaugh NJ (May 1999). "Interactions of prefrontal cortex in relation to awareness in sensory learning". Science. 284 (5419): 1531–3. Bibcode:1999Sci...284.1531M. doi:10.1126/science.284.5419.1531. PMID 10348741.
- Ptito A, Leh SE (October 2007). "Neural substrates of blindsight after hemispherectomy". The Neuroscientist. 13 (5): 506–18. doi:10.1177/1073858407300598. PMID 17901259. S2CID 23093266.
- Ratey JJ, Galaburda AM (2002). A User's Guide to the Brain: Perception, Attention, and the Four Theaters of the Brain. Vintage Books. p. 99. ISBN 978-0-375-70107-8. Archived from the original on 2024-07-26. Retrieved 2018-02-05.
- Beltramo R, Scanziani M (January 2019). "A collicular visual cortex: Neocortical space for an ancient midbrain visual structure". Science. 363 (6422): 64–69. Bibcode:2019Sci...363...64B. doi:10.1126/science.aau7052. PMID 30606842.
External links
- Blind man navigates maze
- Blind man avoids obstacles when reaching
Blindsight is the ability of people who are cortically blind to respond to visual stimuli that they do not consciously see due to lesions in the primary visual cortex also known as the striate cortex or Brodmann Area 17 The term was coined by Lawrence Weiskrantz and his colleagues in a paper published in a 1974 issue of Brain A previous paper studying the discriminatory capacity of a cortically blind patient was published in Nature in 1973 The assumed existence of blindsight is controversial with some arguing that it is merely degraded conscious vision Type classificationThe majority of studies on blindsight are conducted on patients who are hemianopic i e blind in one half of their visual field Following the destruction of the left or right striate cortex patients are asked to detect localize and discriminate amongst visual stimuli that are presented to their blind side often in a forced response or guessing situation even though they may not consciously recognize the visual stimulus Research shows that such blind patients may achieve a higher accuracy than would be expected from chance alone citation needed Type 1 blindsight is the term given to this ability to guess at levels significantly above chance aspects of a visual stimulus such as location or type of movement without any conscious awareness of any stimuli Type 2 blindsight occurs when patients claim to have a feeling that there has been a change within their blind area e g movement but that it was not a visual percept The re classification of blindsight into Type 1 and Type 2 was made after it was shown that the most celebrated blindsight patient GY was usually conscious of stimuli presented to his blind field if the stimuli had certain specific characteristics namely being of high contrast and moving fast at speeds in excess of 20 degrees per second In the aftermath of the First World War a neurologist George Riddoch had described patients who had been blinded by gunshot wounds to V1 who could not see stationary objects but who were as he reported conscious of seeing moving objects in their blind field It is for this reason that the phenomenon has more recently also been called the Riddoch syndrome Since then it has become apparent that such subjects can also become aware of visual stimuli belonging to other visual domains such as color and luminance when presented to their blind fields The ability of such hemianopic subjects to become consciously aware of stimuli presented to their blind field is also commonly referred to as residual or degraded vision As originally defined blindsight challenged the common belief that perceptions must enter consciousness to affect our behavior by showing that our behavior can be guided by sensory information of which we have no conscious awareness Since the demonstration that blind patients can experience some visual stimuli consciously and the consequent redefinition of blindsight into Type 1 and Type 2 a more nuanced view of the phenomenon has developed Blindsight may be thought of as a converse of the form of anosognosia known as Anton syndrome in which there is full cortical blindness along with the confabulation of visual experience HistoryMuch of our current understanding of blindsight can be attributed to early experiments on monkeys One monkey named Helen could be considered the star monkey in visual research because she was the original blindsight subject Helen was a macaque monkey that had been decorticated specifically her primary visual cortex V1 was completely removed blinding her Nevertheless under certain specific situations Helen exhibited sighted behavior Her pupils would dilate and she would blink at stimuli that threatened her eyes Furthermore under certain experimental conditions she could detect a variety of visual stimuli such as the presence and location of objects as well as shape pattern orientation motion and color page needed In many cases she was able to navigate her environment and interact with objects as if she were sighted A similar phenomenon was also discovered in humans Subjects who had suffered damage to their visual cortices due to accidents or strokes reported partial or total blindness Despite this when prompted they could guess the presence and details of objects with above average accuracy and much like animal subjects could catch objects tossed at them The subjects never developed any kind of confidence in their abilities Even when told of their successes they would not begin to spontaneously make guesses about objects but instead still required prompting Furthermore blindsight subjects rarely express the amazement about their abilities that sighted people would expect them to express Describing blindsightPatients with blindsight have damage to the system that produces visual perception the visual cortex of the brain and some of the nerve fibers that bring information to it from the eyes rather than to the underlying brain system controlling eye movements The phenomenon was originally thought to show how after the more complex perception system is damaged people can use the underlying control system to guide hand movements towards an object even though they cannot see what they are reaching for Hence visual information can control behavior without producing a conscious sensation This ability of those with blindsight to act as if able to see objects that they are unconscious of suggested that consciousness is not a general property of all parts of the brain but is produced by specialized parts of it Blindsight patients show awareness of single visual features such as edges and motion but cannot gain a holistic visual percept This suggests that perceptual awareness is modular and that in sighted individuals there is a binding process that unifies all information into a whole percept which is interrupted in patients with such conditions as blindsight and visual agnosia Therefore object identification and object recognition are thought to be separate processes and occur in different areas of the brain working independently from one another The modular theory of object perception and integration would account for the hidden perception experienced in blindsight patients Research has shown that visual stimuli with the single visual features of sharp borders sharp onset offset times motion and low spatial frequency contribute to but are not strictly necessary for an object s salience in blindsight CauseThere are three theories for the explanation of blindsight The first states that after damage to area V1 other branches of the optic nerve deliver visual information to the superior colliculus pulvinar and several other areas including parts of the cerebral cortex In turn these areas might then control the blindsight responses Another explanation for the phenomenon of blindsight is that even though the majority of a person s visual cortex may be damaged tiny islands of functioning tissue remain These islands are not large enough to provide conscious perception but nevertheless enough for some unconscious visual perception A third theory is that the information required to determine the distance to and velocity of an object in object space is determined by the lateral geniculate nucleus LGN before the information is projected to the visual cortex In a normal subject these signals are used to merge the information from the eyes into a three dimensional representation which includes the position and velocity of individual objects relative to the organism extract a vergence signal to benefit the precision previously auxiliary optical system and extract a focus control signal for the lenses of the eyes The stereoscopic information is attached to the object information passed to the visual cortex More recently with the demonstration of a direct input from the LGN to area V5 MT which delivers signals from fast moving stimuli at latencies of about 30 ms another explanation has emerged This one proposes that the delivery of these signals is sufficient to arouse a conscious experience of fast visual motion without implying that it is V5 alone that is responsible since once signals reach V5 they may be propagated to other areas of the brain The latter account would seem to exclude the possibility that signals are pre processed by V1 or post processed by it through return connections from V5 back to V1 as has been suggested The pulvinar nucleus of the thalamus also sends direct V1 by passing signals to V5 but their precise role in generating a conscious visual experience of motion has not yet been determined Evidence of blindsight can be indirectly observed in children as young as two months although there is difficulty in determining the type in a patient who is not old enough to answer questions Evidence in animalsIn a 1995 experiment researchers attempted to show that monkeys with lesions in or even wholly removed striate cortexes also experienced blindsight To study this they had the monkeys complete tasks similar to those commonly used for human subjects The monkeys were placed in front of a monitor and taught to indicate whether a stationary object or nothing was present in their visual field when a tone was played Then the monkeys performed the same task except the stationary objects were presented outside of their visual field The monkeys performed very similar to human participants and were unable to perceive the presence of stationary objects outside of their visual field Another 1995 study by the same group sought to prove that monkeys could also be conscious of movement in their deficit visual field despite not being consciously aware of the presence of an object there To do this researchers used another standard test for humans which was similar to the previous study except moving objects were presented in the deficit visual field Starting from the center of the deficit visual field the object would either move up down or to the right The monkeys performed identically to humans on the test getting them right almost every time This showed that the monkey s ability to detect movement is separate from their ability to consciously detect an object in their deficit visual field and gave further evidence for the claim that damage to the striate cortex plays a large role in causing the disorder Several years later another study compared and contrasted the data collected from monkeys and that of a specific human patient with blindsight GY GY s striate cortical region was damaged through trauma at the age of eight though for the most part he retained full functionality GY was not consciously aware of anything in his right visual field In the monkeys the striate cortex of the left hemisphere was surgically removed By comparing the test results of both GY and the monkeys the researchers concluded that similar patterns of responses to stimuli in the blind visual field can be found in both species ResearchLawrence Weiskrantz and colleagues showed in the early 1970s that if forced to guess about whether a stimulus is present in their blind field some observers do better than chance page needed This ability to detect stimuli that the observer is not conscious of can extend to discrimination of the type of stimulus for example whether an X or O has been presented in the blind field citation needed Electrophysiological evidence from the late 1970s has shown that there is no direct retinal input from S cones to the superior colliculus implying that the perception of color information should be impaired However more recent evidence point to a pathway from S cones to the superior colliculus opposing previous research and supporting the idea that some chromatic processing mechanisms are intact in blindsight Patients shown images on their blind side of people expressing emotions correctly guessed the emotion most of the time The movement of facial muscles used in smiling and frowning were measured and reacted in ways that matched the kind of emotion in the unseen image Therefore the emotions were recognized without involving conscious sight A 2011 study found that a young woman with a unilateral lesion of area V1 could scale her grasping movement as she reached out to pick up objects of different sizes placed in her blind field even though she could not report the sizes of the objects Similarly another patient with unilateral lesion of area V1 could avoid obstacles placed in his blind field when he reached toward a target that was visible in his intact visual field Even though he avoided the obstacles he never reported seeing them A study reported in 2008 asked patient GY to misstate where in his visual field a distinctive stimulus was presented If the stimulus was in the upper part of his visual field he was to say it was in the lower part and vice versa He was able to misstate as requested in his left visual field with normal conscious vision but he tended to fail in the task to state the location correctly when the stimulus was in his blindsight right visual field This failure rate worsened when the stimulus was clearer indicating that failure was not simply due to unreliability of blindsight Case studies This section s tone or style may not reflect the encyclopedic tone used on Wikipedia See Wikipedia s guide to writing better articles for suggestions January 2018 Learn how and when to remove this message DB Researchers applied the same type of tests that were used to study blindsight in animals to a patient referred to as DB The normal techniques used to assess visual acuity in humans involved asking them to verbally describe some visually recognizable aspect of an object or objects DB was given forced choice tasks to complete instead The results of DB s guesses showed that DB was able to determine shape and detect movement at some unconscious level despite not being visually aware of this DB himself chalked up the accuracy of his guesses to be merely coincidental The discovery of the condition known as blindsight raised questions about how different types of visual information even unconscious information may be affected and sometimes even unaffected by damage to different areas of the visual cortex Previous studies had already demonstrated that even without conscious awareness of visual stimuli humans could still determine certain visual features such as presence in the visual field shape orientation and movement But in a newer study evidence showed that if damage to the visual cortex occurs in areas above the primary visual cortex the conscious awareness of visual stimuli itself is not damaged Blindsight shows that even when the primary visual cortex is damaged or removed a person can still perform actions guided by unconscious visual information Despite damage occurring in the area necessary for conscious awareness of visual information other functions of the processing of these visual percepts are still available to the individual The same also goes for damage to other areas of the visual cortex If an area of the cortex that is responsible for a certain function is damaged it will only result in the loss of that particular function or aspect functions that other parts of the visual cortex are responsible for remain intact Alexander and Cowey Alexander and Cowey investigated how contrasting stimuli brightness affects blindsight patients ability to discern movement Prior studies have already shown that blindsight patients are able to detect motion even though they claim they do not see any visual percepts in their blind fields The study subjects were two patients who suffered from hemianopsia blindness in more than half of their visual field Both subjects had displayed the ability to accurately determine the presence of visual stimuli in their blind hemifields without acknowledging an actual visual percept previously To test the effect of brightness on the subject s ability to determine motion they used a white background with a series of colored dots The contrast of the brightness of the dots compared to the white background was altered in each trial to determine if the participants performed better or worse when there was a larger discrepancy in brightness or not The subjects focused on the display for two equal length time intervals and were asked whether they thought the dots were moving during the first or the second time interval When the contrast in brightness between the background and the dots was higher both of the subjects could discern motion more accurately than they would have statistically through guesswork However one subject was not able to accurately determine whether or not blue dots were moving regardless of the brightness contrast but he she was able to do so with every other color dot When the contrast was highest subjects were able to tell whether or not the dots were moving with very high rates of accuracy Even when the dots were white but still of a different brightness from the background subjects could still determine whether they were moving But regardless of the dots color subjects could not tell when they were in motion when the white background and the dots were of similar brightness Kentridge Heywood and Weiskrantz Kentridge Heywood and Weiskrantz used the phenomenon of blindsight to investigate the connection between visual attention and visual awareness They wanted to see if their subject who exhibited blindsight in other studies could react more quickly when their attention was cued without the ability to be visually aware of it The researchers aimed to show that being conscious of a stimulus and paying attention to it was not the same thing To test the relationship between attention and awareness they had the participant try to determine where a target was and whether it was oriented horizontally or vertically on a computer screen The target line would appear at one of two different locations and would be oriented in one of two directions Before the target would appear an arrow would become visible on the screen sometimes pointing to the correct position of the target line and less frequently not This arrow was the cue for the subject The participant would press a key to indicate whether the line was horizontal or vertical and could then also indicate to an observer whether or not he she actually had a feeling that any object was there or not even if they couldn t see anything The participant was able to accurately determine the orientation of the line when the target was cued by an arrow before the appearance of the target even though these visual stimuli did not equal awareness in the subject who had no vision in that area of his her visual field The study showed that even without the ability to be visually aware of a stimulus the participant could still focus his her attention on this object TN In 2003 a patient known as TN lost use of his primary visual cortex area V1 He had two successive strokes which knocked out the region in both his left and right hemispheres After his strokes ordinary tests of TN s sight turned up nothing He could not even detect large objects moving right in front of his eyes Researchers eventually began to notice that TN exhibited signs of blindsight and in 2008 decided to test their theory They took TN into a hallway and asked him to walk through it without using the cane he always carried after having the strokes TN was not aware at the time but the researchers had placed various obstacles in the hallway to test if he could avoid them without conscious use of his sight To the researchers delight he moved around every obstacle with ease at one point even pressing himself up against the wall to squeeze past a trashcan placed in his way After navigating through the hallway TN reported that he was just walking the way he wanted to not because he knew anything was there Mr J In another case study a girl brought her grandfather in to see a neuropsychologist The girl s grandfather Mr J had suffered a stroke that had left him completely blind apart from a tiny spot in the middle of his visual field The neuropsychologist Dr M performed an exercise with him The doctor helped Mr J to a chair had him sit down and then asked to borrow his cane The doctor then asked Mr J please look straight ahead Keep looking that way and don t move your eyes or turn your head I know that you can see a little bit straight ahead of you and I don t want you to use that piece of vision for what I m going to ask you to do Fine Now I d like you to reach out with your right hand and point to what I m holding Mr J then replied But I don t see anything I m blind The doctor then said I know but please try anyway Mr J then shrugged and pointed and was surprised when his finger encountered the end of the cane which the doctor was pointing toward him After this Mr J said that it was just luck The doctor then turned the cane around so that the handle side was pointing towards Mr J He then asked for Mr J to grab hold of the cane Mr J reached out with an open hand and grabbed hold of the cane After this the doctor said Good Now put your hand down please The doctor then rotated the cane 90 degrees so that the handle was oriented vertically The doctor then asked Mr J to reach for the cane again Mr J did this turning his wrist so that his hand matched the orientation of the handle This case study shows that although on a conscious level Mr J was completely unaware of any visual abilities that he may have had he was able to orient his grabbing motions as if he had no visual impairments Other cases Other cases refer to SL GY and GR Brain regions involvedThis section needs more reliable medical references for verification or relies too heavily on primary sources specifically only final sentence appears to be inline referenced Please review the contents of the section and add the appropriate references if you can Unsourced or poorly sourced material may be challenged and removed Find sources Blindsight news newspapers books scholar JSTOR August 2015 Visual processing in the brain goes through a series of stages Destruction of the primary visual cortex leads to blindness in the part of the visual field that corresponds to the damaged cortical representation The area of blindness known as a scotoma is in the visual field opposite the damaged hemisphere and can vary from a small area up to the entire hemifield Visual processing occurs in the brain in a hierarchical series of stages with much crosstalk and feedback between areas The route from the retina through V1 is not the only visual pathway into the cortex though it is by far the largest it is commonly thought that the residual performance of people exhibiting blindsight is due to preserved pathways into the extrastriate cortex that bypass V1 However both physiological evidence in monkeys and behavioral and imaging evidence in humans shows that activity in these extrastriate areas and especially in V5 is apparently sufficient to support visual awareness in the absence of V1 To put it in a more complex way recent physiological findings suggest that visual processing takes place along several independent parallel pathways One system processes information about shape one about color and one about movement location and spatial organization This information moves through an area of the brain called the lateral geniculate nucleus located in the thalamus and on to be processed in the primary visual cortex area V1 also known as the striate cortex because of its striped appearance People with damage to V1 report no conscious vision no visual imagery and no visual images in their dreams However some of these people still experience the blindsight phenomenon though this too is controversial with some studies showing a limited amount of consciousness without V1 or projections relating to it The superior colliculus and prefrontal cortex also have a major role in awareness of a visual stimulus Lateral geniculate nucleus Mosby s Dictionary of Medicine Nursing amp Health Professions defines the LGN as one of two elevations of the lateral posterior thalamus receiving visual impulses from the retina via the optic nerves and tracts and relaying the impulses to the calcarine visual cortex What is seen in the left and right visual field is taken in by each eye and brought back to the optic disc via the nerve fibres of the retina From the optic disc visual information travels through the optic nerve and into the optic chiasm Visual information then enters the optic tract and travels to four different areas of the brain including the superior colliculus pretectum of the mid brain the suprachiasmatic nucleus of the hypothalamus and the lateral geniculate nucleus LGN Most axons from the LGN will then travel to the primary visual cortex Injury to the primary visual cortex including lesions and other trauma leads to the loss of visual experience However the residual vision that is left cannot be attributed to V1 According to Schmid et al thalamic lateral geniculate nucleus has a causal role in V1 independent processing of visual information This information was found through experiments using fMRI during activation and inactivation of the LGN and the contribution the LGN has on visual experience in monkeys with a V1 lesion These researchers concluded that the magnocellular system of the LGN is less affected by the removal of V1 which suggests that it is because of this system in the LGN that blindsight occurs Furthermore once the LGN was inactivated virtually all of the extrastriate areas of the brain no longer showed a response on the fMRI The information leads to a qualitative assessment that included scotoma stimulation with the LGN intact had fMRI activation of 20 of that under normal conditions This finding agrees with the information obtained from and fMRI images of patients with blindsight The same study also supported the conclusion that the LGN plays a substantial role in blindsight Specifically while injury to V1 does create a loss of vision the LGN is less affected and may result in the residual vision that remains causing the sight in blindsight Functional magnetic resonance imaging has launched has also been employed to conduct brain scans in normal healthy human volunteers to attempt to demonstrate that visual motion can bypass V1 through a connection from the LGN to the human middle temporal complex Their findings concluded that there was an indeed a connection of visual motion information that went directly from the LGN to the V5 hMT bypassing V1 completely Evidence also suggests that following a traumatic injury to V1 there is still a direct pathway from the retina through the LGN to the extrastriate visual areas The extrastriate visual areas include parts of the occipital lobe that surround V1 In non human primates these often include V2 V3 and V4 In a study conducted in primates after partial ablation of area V1 areas V2 and V3 were still excited by visual stimulus Other evidence suggests that the LGN projections that survive V1 removal are relatively sparse in density but are nevertheless widespread and probably encompass all extrastriate visual areas including V2 V4 V5 and the inferotemporal cortex region ControversyThe results of some experiments suggest that blindsighted people may be preserving some kind of conscious experience and thus they are not fully blind The criteria for blindsight has repeatedly changed based on findings that challenge the original definition which has led some scientists to cast doubt on the existence of blindsight See alsoKoniocellular cell Riddoch syndrome Two streams hypothesis Visual agnosiaReferencesCelesia G 2010 Visual perception and awareness a modular system Journal of Psychophysiology 24 2 62 67 doi 10 1027 0269 8803 a000014 Weiskrantz L Warrington EK Sanders MD Marshall J December 1974 Visual capacity in the hemianopic field following a restricted occipital ablation Brain 97 4 709 28 doi 10 1093 brain 97 1 709 PMID 4434190 Poppel E Held R Frost D June 1973 Leter Residual visual function after brain wounds involving the central visual pathways in man Nature 243 5405 295 6 doi 10 1038 243295a0 PMID 4774871 S2CID 4160116 Overgaard Morten November 2012 Blindsight recent and historical controversies on the blindness of blindsight WIREs Cognitive Science 3 6 607 614 doi 10 1002 wcs 1194 ISSN 1939 5078 PMID 26305269 Overgaard M Fehl K Mouridsen K Bergholt B Cleeremans A August 2008 Seeing without Seeing Degraded Conscious Vision in a Blindsight Patient PLOS ONE 3 8 e3028 Bibcode 2008PLoSO 3 3028O doi 10 1371 journal pone 0003028 PMC 2507770 PMID 18716654 Mazzi C Bagattini C Savazzi S 2016 Blind Sight vs Degraded Sight Different Measures Tell a Different Story Frontiers in Psychology 7 901 doi 10 3389 fpsyg 2016 00901 PMC 4909743 PMID 27378993 Weiskrantz L 1997 Consciousness Lost and Found A Neuropsychological Exploration Oxford University Press ISBN 978 0 19 852301 7 Barbur JL Watson JD Frackowiak RS Zeki S December 1993 Conscious visual perception without V1 Brain 116 Pt 6 6 1293 302 doi 10 1093 brain 116 6 1293 PMID 8293272 Stoerig Petra Barth Erhardt 2001 12 01 Low Level Phenomenal Vision Despite Unilateral Destruction of Primary Visual Cortex Consciousness and Cognition 10 4 574 587 doi 10 1006 ccog 2001 0526 ISSN 1053 8100 PMID 11790045 S2CID 22895605 Archived from the original on 2021 04 24 Retrieved 2021 04 24 Riddoch G 1917 05 01 Dissociation of Visual Perceptions Due to Occipital Injuries With Especial Reference to Appreciation of movement Brain 40 1 15 57 doi 10 1093 brain 40 1 15 ISSN 0006 8950 Zeki S Ffytche DH January 1998 The Riddoch syndrome insights into the neurobiology of conscious vision Brain 121 Pt 1 1 25 45 doi 10 1093 brain 121 1 25 PMID 9549486 Morland AB Jones SR Finlay AL Deyzac E Le S Kemp S June 1999 Visual perception of motion luminance and colour in a human hemianope Brain 122 Pt 6 6 1183 98 doi 10 1093 brain 122 6 1183 PMID 10356069 Carlson N 2013 Physiology of Behavior 11th ed University of Massachusetts Amherst Pearson Education Inc p 4 ISBN 978 0 205 23981 8 Weisberg Josh June 2006 Red in the head Nature 441 7093 577 Bibcode 2006Natur 441 577W doi 10 1038 441577a ISSN 1476 4687 S2CID 26642958 Humphrey NK 1970 What the frog s eye tells the monkey s brain Brain Behavior and Evolution 3 1 324 37 doi 10 1159 000125480 PMID 5001242 Humphrey NK 1974 Vision in a monkey without striate cortex a case study Perception 3 3 241 55 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 452 5493 doi 10 1068 p030241 PMID 4459818 S2CID 4686081 Humphrey N 1992 A History of the Mind New York Simon amp Schuster ISBN 9780671686444 OCLC 25915998 Holt J 2003 Blindsight and the Nature of Consciousness Peterborough Ontario Broadview Press ISBN 978 1 55111 351 7 OCLC 50755257 page needed Humphrey N 2006 Seeing Red A Study in Consciousness Mind brain behavior initiative Cambridge Massachusetts Belknap Press ISBN 978 0 674 02179 2 OCLC 234101445 page needed Alexander I Cowey A June 2010 Edges colour and awareness in blindsight Consciousness and Cognition 19 2 520 33 doi 10 1016 j concog 2010 01 008 PMID 20171122 S2CID 36139700 Ffytche DH Zeki S January 2011 The primary visual cortex and feedback to it are not necessary for conscious vision Brain 134 Pt 1 247 57 doi 10 1093 brain awq305 PMC 3159156 PMID 21097490 Sahraie A Hibbard PB Trevethan CT Ritchie KL Weiskrantz L December 2010 Consciousness of the first order in blindsight Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 107 49 21217 22 Bibcode 2010PNAS 10721217S doi 10 1073 pnas 1015652107 PMC 3000284 PMID 21078979 Kinoshita M Kato R Isa K Kobayashi K Kobayashi K Onoe H Isa T 2019 01 11 Dissecting the circuit for blindsight to reveal the critical role of pulvinar and superior colliculus Nat Commun 10 1 135 Bibcode 2019NatCo 10 135K doi 10 1038 s41467 018 08058 0 PMC 6329824 PMID 30635570 S2CID 58009143 Kletenik I Ferguson MA Bateman JR Cohen AL Lin C Tetreault A Pelak VS Anderson CA Prasad S Darby RR Fox MD 2021 12 27 Network Localization of Unconscious Visual Perception in Blindsight Ann Neurol 91 2 217 224 doi 10 1002 ana 26292 PMC 10013845 PMID 34961965 S2CID 245553461 Radoeva PD Prasad S Brainard DH Aguirre GK 2008 11 20 Neural activity within area V1 reflects unconscious visual performance in a case of blindsight J Cogn Neurosci 20 11 1927 1939 doi 10 1162 jocn 2008 20139 PMC 2773243 PMID 18416678 Kalat JW 2009 Biological Psychology 10th ed Belmont California Wadsworth pp 169 170 ISBN 9780495603009 OCLC 236316740 Fulton J 2004 Processes in Biological Vision Section 7 4 Archived copy PDF Archived from the original on 2015 02 21 Retrieved 2012 11 26 a href wiki Template Cite web title Template Cite web cite web a CS1 maint archived copy as title link Benevento LA Yoshida K December 1981 The afferent and efferent organization of the lateral geniculo prestriate pathways in the macaque monkey The Journal of Comparative Neurology 203 3 455 74 doi 10 1002 cne 902030309 PMID 6274921 S2CID 28585691 Fries W September 1981 The projection from the lateral geniculate nucleus to the prestriate cortex of the macaque monkey Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B Biological Sciences 213 1190 73 86 Bibcode 1981RSPSB 213 73F doi 10 1098 rspb 1981 0054 PMID 6117869 S2CID 5700048 Yukie M Iwai E September 1981 Direct projection from the dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus to the prestriate cortex in macaque monkeys The Journal of Comparative Neurology 201 1 81 97 doi 10 1002 cne 902010107 PMID 7276252 S2CID 8825689 Sincich LC Park KF Wohlgemuth MJ Horton JC October 2004 Bypassing V1 a direct geniculate input to area MT Nature Neuroscience 7 10 1123 8 doi 10 1038 nn1318 PMID 15378066 S2CID 13419990 ffytche DH Guy CN Zeki S December 1995 The parallel visual motion inputs into areas V1 and V5 of human cerebral cortex Brain 118 Pt 6 6 1375 94 doi 10 1093 brain 118 6 1375 PMID 8595471 Beckers G Zeki S February 1995 The consequences of inactivating areas V1 and V5 on visual motion perception Brain 118 Pt 1 1 49 60 doi 10 1093 brain 118 1 49 PMID 7895014 Schmid MC Mrowka SW Turchi J Saunders RC Wilke M Peters AJ et al July 2010 Blindsight depends on the lateral geniculate nucleus Nature 466 7304 373 7 Bibcode 2010Natur 466 373S doi 10 1038 nature09179 PMC 2904843 PMID 20574422 Ffytche DH Zeki S January 2011 The primary visual cortex and feedback to it are not necessary for conscious vision Brain 134 Pt 1 247 57 doi 10 1093 brain awq305 PMC 3159156 PMID 21097490 Lamme VAF 2001 04 01 Blindsight the role of feedforward and feedback corticocortical connections Acta Psychologica 107 1 3 209 228 doi 10 1016 S0001 6918 01 00020 8 ISSN 0001 6918 PMID 11388136 Archived from the original on 2021 04 27 Retrieved 2021 04 09 Cragg B G 1969 07 01 The topography of the afferent projections in the circumstriate visual cortex of the monkey studied by the nauta method Vision Research 9 7 733 747 doi 10 1016 0042 6989 69 90011 X ISSN 0042 6989 PMID 4979024 Archived from the original on 2022 08 13 Retrieved 2021 04 09 Boyle NJ Jones DH Hamilton R Spowart KM Dutton GN October 2005 Blindsight in children does it exist and can it be used to help the child Observations on a case series Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology 47 10 699 702 doi 10 1111 j 1469 8749 2005 tb01057 x PMID 16174315 Cowey A Stoerig P January 1995 Blindsight in monkeys PDF Nature 373 6511 247 9 Bibcode 1995Natur 373 247C doi 10 1038 373247a0 PMID 7816139 S2CID 4269412 Archived from the original PDF on 2012 05 01 Retrieved 2018 02 05 Stoerig P Cowey A March 1997 Blindsight in man and monkey Brain 120 Pt 3 3 535 59 doi 10 1093 brain 120 3 535 PMID 9126063 dead link Cowey A Alexander I Stoerig P February 2008 A blindsight conundrum how to respond when there isn t a correct response Neuropsychologia 46 3 870 8 doi 10 1016 j neuropsychologia 2007 11 031 PMID 18201733 S2CID 20790934 Weiskrantz L 1986 Blindsight A Case Study and Implications Oxford University Press ISBN 978 0 19 852192 1 OCLC 21677307 de Monasterio FM November 1978 Properties of ganglion cells with atypical receptive field organization in retina of macaques Journal of Neurophysiology 41 6 1435 49 doi 10 1152 jn 1978 41 6 1435 PMID 104014 Marrocco RT Li RH July 1977 Monkey superior colliculus properties of single cells and their afferent inputs Journal of Neurophysiology 40 4 844 60 doi 10 1152 jn 1977 40 4 844 PMID 407333 Schiller PH Malpeli JG March 1977 Properties and tectal projections of monkey retinal ganglion cells Journal of Neurophysiology 40 2 428 45 doi 10 1152 jn 1977 40 2 428 PMID 403252 Hall NJ Colby CL 2009 Response to blue visual stimuli in the macaque superior colliculus Society for Neuroscience 19 520 533 Garrett B 2011 Brain amp Behavior An Introduction to Biological Psychology 3rd ed Thousand Oaks California SAGE Publications pp 315 318 ISBN 978 1 4129 8168 2 OCLC 617425474 Tamietto M de Gelder B October 2010 Neural bases of the non conscious perception of emotional signals Nature Reviews Neuroscience 11 10 697 709 doi 10 1038 nrn2889 hdl 2318 79483 PMID 20811475 S2CID 4690318 Whitwell RL Striemer CL Nicolle DA Goodale MA April 2011 Grasping the non conscious preserved grip scaling to unseen objects for immediate but not delayed grasping following a unilateral lesion to primary visual cortex Vision Research 51 8 908 24 doi 10 1016 j visres 2011 02 005 PMID 21324336 Striemer CL Chapman CS Goodale MA September 2009 Real time obstacle avoidance in the absence of primary visual cortex Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 106 37 15996 6001 Bibcode 2009PNAS 10615996S doi 10 1073 pnas 0905549106 PMC 2747232 PMID 19805240 Persaud N Cowey A September 2008 Blindsight is unlike normal conscious vision evidence from an exclusion task Consciousness and Cognition 17 3 1050 5 doi 10 1016 j concog 2007 10 002 PMID 18065242 S2CID 30699219 Weiskrantz L 2007 Blindsight Scholarpedia 2 4 3047 Bibcode 2007SchpJ 2 3047W doi 10 4249 scholarpedia 3047 Stoerig P September 1996 Varieties of vision from blind responses to conscious recognition Trends in Neurosciences 19 9 401 6 doi 10 1016 S0166 2236 96 10051 5 PMID 8873358 S2CID 25012895 Alexander I Cowey A March 2013 Isoluminant coloured stimuli are undetectable in blindsight even when they move Experimental Brain Research 225 1 147 52 doi 10 1007 s00221 012 3355 6 PMID 23263562 S2CID 1738371 Kentridge RW Heywood CA Weiskrantz L 2004 Spatial attention speeds discrimination without awareness in blindsight Neuropsychologia 42 6 831 5 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 719 7118 doi 10 1016 j neuropsychologia 2003 11 001 PMID 15037061 S2CID 12837840 de Gelder B Tamietto M van Boxtel G Goebel R Sahraie A van den Stock J et al December 2008 Intact navigation skills after bilateral loss of striate cortex Current Biology 18 24 R1128 9 Bibcode 2008CBio 18R1128D doi 10 1016 j cub 2008 11 002 PMID 19108766 Rodman HR Gross CG Albright TD June 1989 Afferent basis of visual response properties in area MT of the macaque I Effects of striate cortex removal The Journal of Neuroscience 9 6 2033 50 doi 10 1523 JNEUROSCI 09 06 02033 1989 PMC 6569731 PMID 2723765 Gaglianese A Costagli M Bernardi G Ricciardi E Pietrini P April 2012 Evidence of a direct influence between the thalamus and hMT independent of V1 in the human brain as measured by fMRI NeuroImage 60 2 1440 7 doi 10 1016 j neuroimage 2012 01 093 PMID 22300813 S2CID 937762 Ffytche DH Zeki S January 2011 The primary visual cortex and feedback to it are not necessary for conscious vision Brain 134 Pt 1 247 57 doi 10 1093 brain awq305 PMC 3159156 PMID 21097490 Mosby s Dictionary of Medicine Nursing amp Health Professions Mosby Inc 8th ed St Louis Missouri Mosby Elsevier 2009 ISBN 9780323049375 OCLC 226911727 a href wiki Template Cite book title Template Cite book cite book a CS1 maint others link Dragoi V 1997 Chapter 15 Visual Processing Cortical Pathways Neuroscience Online The University of Texas Health Science Center at Houston Archived from the original on July 26 2024 Retrieved November 3 2013 Cowey A September 2010 Visual system how does blindsight arise Current Biology 20 17 R702 4 Bibcode 2010CBio 20 R702C doi 10 1016 j cub 2010 07 014 PMID 20833309 S2CID 17351599 Weiskrantz L April 1996 Blindsight revisited Current Opinion in Neurobiology 6 2 215 20 doi 10 1016 s0959 4388 96 80075 4 PMID 8725963 S2CID 1833570 Further reading Blindsight How brain sees what you do not see Medical Press 14 October 2008 Archived from the original on 6 February 2018 Retrieved 5 February 2018 Collins GP Blindsight Seeing without knowing it Scientific American Blog Network Archived from the original on 26 July 2024 Retrieved 5 February 2018 Danckert J Rossetti Y 2005 Blindsight in action what can the different sub types of blindsight tell us about the control of visually guided actions Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 29 7 1035 46 doi 10 1016 j neubiorev 2005 02 001 PMID 16143169 S2CID 12833434 De Gelder B May 2010 Uncanny sight in the blind Scientific American 302 5 60 5 Bibcode 2010SciAm 302e 60D doi 10 1038 scientificamerican0510 60 PMID 20443379 Leh SE Johansen Berg H Ptito A July 2006 Unconscious vision new insights into the neuronal correlate of blindsight using diffusion tractography Brain 129 Pt 7 1822 32 doi 10 1093 brain awl111 PMID 16714319 Leh SE Mullen KT Ptito A November 2006 Absence of S cone input in human blindsight following hemispherectomy The European Journal of Neuroscience 24 10 2954 60 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 578 4900 doi 10 1111 j 1460 9568 2006 05178 x PMID 17156217 S2CID 14152585 McIntosh AR Rajah MN Lobaugh NJ May 1999 Interactions of prefrontal cortex in relation to awareness in sensory learning Science 284 5419 1531 3 Bibcode 1999Sci 284 1531M doi 10 1126 science 284 5419 1531 PMID 10348741 Ptito A Leh SE October 2007 Neural substrates of blindsight after hemispherectomy The Neuroscientist 13 5 506 18 doi 10 1177 1073858407300598 PMID 17901259 S2CID 23093266 Ratey JJ Galaburda AM 2002 A User s Guide to the Brain Perception Attention and the Four Theaters of the Brain Vintage Books p 99 ISBN 978 0 375 70107 8 Archived from the original on 2024 07 26 Retrieved 2018 02 05 Beltramo R Scanziani M January 2019 A collicular visual cortex Neocortical space for an ancient midbrain visual structure Science 363 6422 64 69 Bibcode 2019Sci 363 64B doi 10 1126 science aau7052 PMID 30606842 External linksBlind man navigates maze Blind man avoids obstacles when reaching