Primate

Author: www.NiNa.Az
Jan 31, 2025 / 06:42

Primates is an order of mammals which is further divided into the strepsirrhines which include lemurs galagos and lorisi

Primate
Primate
Primate

Primates is an order of mammals, which is further divided into the strepsirrhines, which include lemurs, galagos, and lorisids; and the haplorhines, which include tarsiers and simians (monkeys and apes). Primates arose 74–63 million years ago first from small terrestrial mammals, which adapted for life in tropical forests: many primate characteristics represent adaptations to the challenging environment among tree tops, including large brain sizes, binocular vision, color vision, vocalizations, shoulder girdles allowing a large degree of movement in the upper limbs, and opposable thumbs (in most but not all) that enable better grasping and dexterity. Primates range in size from Madame Berthe's mouse lemur, which weighs 30 g (1 oz), to the eastern gorilla, weighing over 200 kg (440 lb). There are 376–524 species of living primates, depending on which classification is used. New primate species continue to be discovered: over 25 species were described in the 2000s, 36 in the 2010s, and six in the 2020s.

Primates
Temporal range: 65.9–0 Ma
PreꞒ
O
S
D
C
P
T
J
K
Pg
N
Early Paleocene to Present
image
Ring-tailed lemur
image
Red slender loris
image
Northern greater galago
image
Philippine tarsier
image
Black spider monkey
image
Hamadryas baboon
image
Lar gibbon
Scientific classification image
Domain: Eukaryota
Kingdom: Animalia
Phylum: Chordata
Class: Mammalia
Mirorder: Primatomorpha
Order: Primates
Linnaeus, 1758
Suborders
  • Strepsirrhini
  • Haplorhini
  • Altiatlasius
image
Range and density of non-human primates.
Synonyms

Plesiadapiformes (cladistically including crown primates)

Primates have large brains (relative to body size) compared to other mammals, as well as an increased reliance on visual acuity at the expense of the sense of smell, which is the dominant sensory system in most mammals. These features are more developed in monkeys and apes, and noticeably less so in lorises and lemurs. Some primates, including gorillas, humans and baboons, are primarily ground-dwelling rather than arboreal, but all species have adaptations for climbing trees. Arboreal locomotion techniques used include leaping from tree to tree and swinging between branches of trees (brachiation); terrestrial locomotion techniques include walking on two hindlimbs (bipedalism) and modified walking on four limbs (quadrupedalism) via knuckle-walking.

Primates are among the most social of all animals, forming pairs or family groups, uni-male harems, and multi-male/multi-female groups. Non-human primates have at least four types of social systems, many defined by the amount of movement by adolescent females between groups. Primates have slower rates of development than other similarly sized mammals, reach maturity later, and have longer lifespans. Primates are also the most cognitively advanced animals, with humans (genus Homo) capable of creating complex languages and sophisticated civilizations, and non-human primates are recorded to use tools. They may communicate using facial and hand gestures, smells and vocalizations.

Close interactions between humans and non-human primates (NHPs) can create opportunities for the transmission of zoonotic diseases, especially virus diseases including herpes, measles, ebola, rabies and hepatitis. Thousands of non-human primates are used in research around the world because of their psychological and physiological similarity to humans. About 60% of primate species are threatened with extinction. Common threats include deforestation, forest fragmentation, monkey drives, and primate hunting for use in medicines, as pets, and for food. Large-scale tropical forest clearing for agriculture most threatens primates.

Etymology

The English name primates is derived from Old French or French primat, from a noun use of Latin primat-, from primus ('prime, first rank'). The name was given by Carl Linnaeus because he thought this the "highest" order of animals. The relationships among the different groups of primates were not clearly understood until relatively recently, so the commonly used terms are somewhat confused. For example, ape has been used either as an alternative for monkey or for any tailless, relatively human-like primate.

Sir Wilfrid Le Gros Clark was one of the primatologists who developed the idea of trends in primate evolution and the methodology of arranging the living members of an order into an "ascending series" leading to humans. Commonly used names for groups of primates such as prosimians, monkeys, lesser apes, and great apes reflect this methodology. According to our current understanding of the evolutionary history of the primates, several of these groups are paraphyletic, or rather they do not include all the descendants of a common ancestor.

In contrast with Clark's methodology, modern classifications typically identify (or name) only those groupings that are monophyletic; that is, such a named group includes all the descendants of the group's common ancestor.

All groups with scientific names are clades, or monophyletic groups, and the sequence of scientific classification reflects the evolutionary history of the related lineages. Groups that are traditionally named are shown on the right; they form an "ascending series" (per Clark, see above), and several groups are paraphyletic:

  • Prosimians contain two monophyletic groups (the suborder Strepsirrhini, or lemurs, lorises and allies, as well as the tarsiers of the suborder Haplorhini); it is a paraphyletic grouping because it excludes the Simiiformes, which also are descendants of the common ancestor Primates.
  • Monkeys comprise two monophyletic groups, New World monkeys and Old World monkeys, but is paraphyletic because it excludes hominoids, superfamily Hominoidea, also descendants of the common ancestor Simiiformes.
  • Apes as a whole, and the great apes, are paraphyletic if the terms are used such that they exclude humans.

Thus, the members of the two sets of groups, and hence names, do not match, which causes problems in relating scientific names to common (usually traditional) names. Consider the superfamily Hominoidea: In terms of the common names on the right, this group consists of apes and humans and there is no single common name for all the members of the group. One remedy is to create a new common name, in this case hominoids. Another possibility is to expand the use of one of the traditional names. For example, in his 2005 book, the vertebrate palaeontologist Benton wrote, "The apes, Hominoidea, today include the gibbons and orangutan ... the gorilla and chimpanzee ... and humans"; thereby Benton was using apes to mean hominoids. In that case, the group heretofore called apes must now be identified as the non-human apes.

As of 2021, there is no consensus as to whether to accept traditional (that is, common), but paraphyletic, names or to use monophyletic names only; or to use 'new' common names or adaptations of old ones. Both competing approaches can be found in biological sources, often in the same work, and sometimes by the same author. Thus, Benton defines apes to include humans, then he repeatedly uses ape-like to mean 'like an ape rather than a human'; and when discussing the reaction of others to a new fossil he writes of "claims that Orrorin ... was an ape rather than a human".

Classification of living primates

Order Primates was established by Carl Linnaeus in 1758, in the tenth edition of his book Systema Naturae, for the genera Homo (humans), Simia (other apes and monkeys), Lemur (prosimians) and Vespertilio (bats). In the first edition of the same book (1735), he had used the name Anthropomorpha for Homo, Simia and Bradypus (sloths). In 1839, Henri Marie Ducrotay de Blainville, following Linnaeus and aping his nomenclature, established the orders Secundates (including the suborders Chiroptera, Insectivora and Carnivora), Tertiates (or Glires) and Quaternates (including Gravigrada, Pachydermata and Ruminantia), but these new taxa were not accepted.

Before Anderson and Jones introduced the classification of Strepsirrhini and Haplorhini in 1984, (followed by McKenna and Bell's 1997 work Classification of Mammals: Above the species level), Primates was divided into two superfamilies: Prosimii and Anthropoidea. Prosimii included all of the prosimians: Strepsirrhini plus the tarsiers. Anthropoidea contained all of the simians.

The cladogram below shows one possible classification sequence of the living primates: groups that use common (traditional) names are shown on the right.

Primatomorpha

Dermopteraimage

Primates
Strepsirrhini
Lemuriformes

lemurs (superfamily Lemuroidea)image

lorises and allies (superfamily Lorisoidea)image

Haplorhini
Tarsiiformes

tarsiers (superfamily Tarsioidea)image

Simiiformes

New World monkeys (parvorder Platyrrhini)image

Catarrhini

Old World monkeys (superfamily Cercopithecoidea)image

Hominoidea

gibbons (family Hylobatidae)image

Hominidae

orangutans (subfamily Ponginae)image

Homininae

gorillas (tribe Gorillini)image

Hominini

humans (g. Homo)image

chimpanzees, bonobos (g. Pan)image

prosimians
monkeys
lesser apes
great apes

Phylogeny and genetics

Euarchontoglires  
Glires 

Rodentia (rodents)

Lagomorpha (rabbits, hares, pikas)

 Euarchonta 

Scandentia (treeshrews)

Primatomorpha

Dermoptera (colugos)

Primates

Plesiadapiformes

crown primates

Order Primates is part of the clade Euarchontoglires, which is nested within the clade Eutheria of Class Mammalia. Recent molecular genetic research on primates, colugos, and treeshrews has shown that the two species of colugos are more closely related to primates than to treeshrews, even though treeshrews were at one time considered primates. These three orders make up the clade Euarchonta. The combination of this clade with the clade Glires (composed of Rodentia and Lagomorpha) forms the clade Euarchontoglires. Variously, both Euarchonta and Euarchontoglires are ranked as superorders. Some scientists consider Dermoptera to be a suborder of Primates and use the suborder Euprimates for the "true" primates.

Evolutionary history

The primate lineage is thought to go back at least near the Cretaceous–Paleogene boundary or around 74–63 (mya). The earliest possible primate/proto-primate may be Purgatorius, which dates back to Early Paleocene of North America ~66mya. The oldest known primates from the fossil record date to the Late Paleocene of Africa, c.57 mya (Altiatlasius) or the Paleocene-Eocene transition in the northern continents, c. 55 mya (Cantius, Donrussellia, Altanius, Plesiadapis and Teilhardina). Other studies, including molecular clock studies, have estimated the origin of the primate branch to have been in the mid-Cretaceous period, around 85 mya.

By modern cladistic reckoning, the order Primates is monophyletic. The suborder Strepsirrhini, the "wet-nosed" primates, is generally thought to have split off from the primitive primate line about 63 mya, although earlier dates are also supported. The seven strepsirrhine families are the five related lemur families and the two remaining families that include the lorisids and the galagos. Older classification schemes wrap Lepilemuridae into Lemuridae and Galagidae into Lorisidae, yielding a four-one family distribution instead of five-two as presented here. During the Eocene, most of the northern continents were dominated by two groups, the adapiforms and the omomyids. The former are considered members of Strepsirrhini, but did not have a toothcomb like modern lemurs; recent analysis has demonstrated that Darwinius masillae fits into this grouping. The latter was closely related to tarsiers, monkeys, and apes. How these two groups relate to extant primates is unclear. Omomyids perished about 30 mya, while adapiforms survived until about 10 mya.

According to genetic studies, the lemurs of Madagascar diverged from the lorisoids approximately 75 mya. These studies, as well as chromosomal and molecular evidence, also show that lemurs are more closely related to each other than to other strepsirrhine primates. However, Madagascar split from Africa 160 mya and from India 90 mya. To account for these facts, a founding lemur population of a few individuals is thought to have reached Madagascar from Africa via a single rafting event between 50 and 80 mya. Other colonization options have been suggested, such as multiple colonizations from Africa and India, but none are supported by the genetic and molecular evidence.

image
Common brown lemur, a strepsirrhine primate

Until recently, the aye-aye has been difficult to place within Strepsirrhini. Theories had been proposed that its family, Daubentoniidae, was either a lemuriform primate (meaning its ancestors split from the lemur line more recently than lemurs and lorises split) or a sister group to all the other strepsirrhines. In 2008, the aye-aye family was confirmed to be most closely related to the other Malagasy lemurs, likely having descended from the same ancestral population that colonized the island.

Suborder Haplorhini, the simple-nosed or "dry-nosed" primates, is composed of two sister clades.Prosimian tarsiers in the family Tarsiidae (monotypic in its own infraorder Tarsiiformes), represent the most basal division, originating about 58 mya. The earliest known haplorhine skeleton, that of 55 MA old tarsier-like Archicebus, was found in central China, supporting an already suspected Asian origin for the group. The infraorder Simiiformes (simian primates, consisting of monkeys and apes) emerged about 40 mya, possibly also in Asia; if so, they dispersed across the Tethys Sea from Asia to Africa soon afterwards. There are two simian clades, both parvorders: Catarrhini, which developed in Africa, consisting of Old World monkeys, humans and the other apes, and Platyrrhini, which developed in South America, consisting of New World monkeys. A third clade, which included the eosimiids, developed in Asia, but became extinct millions of years ago.

As in the case of lemurs, the origin of New World monkeys is unclear. Molecular studies of concatenated nuclear sequences have yielded a widely varying estimated date of divergence between platyrrhines and catarrhines, ranging from 33 to 70 mya, while studies based on mitochondrial sequences produce a narrower range of 35 to 43 mya. The anthropoid primates possibly traversed the Atlantic Ocean from Africa to South America during the Eocene by island hopping, facilitated by Atlantic Ocean ridges and a lowered sea level. Alternatively, a single rafting event may explain this transoceanic colonization. Due to continental drift, the Atlantic Ocean was not nearly as wide at the time as it is today. Research suggests that a small 1 kg (2.2 lb) primate could have survived 13 days on a raft of vegetation. Given estimated current and wind speeds, this would have provided enough time to make the voyage between the continents.

image
Emperor tamarin, a New World monkey

Apes and monkeys spread from Africa into Europe and Asia starting in the Miocene. Soon after, the lorises and tarsiers made the same journey. The first hominin fossils were discovered in northern Africa and date back 5–8 mya. Old World monkeys disappeared from Europe about 1.8 mya. Molecular and fossil studies generally show that modern humans originated in Africa 100,000–200,000 years ago.

Although primates are well studied in comparison to other animal groups, several new species have been discovered recently, and genetic tests have revealed previously unrecognised species in known populations. Primate Taxonomy listed about 350 species of primates in 2001; the author, Colin Groves, increased that number to 376 for his contribution to the third edition of Mammal Species of the World (MSW3). However, publications since the taxonomy in MSW3 was compiled in 2003 have pushed the number to 522 species, or 708 including subspecies.

Hybrids

Primate hybrids usually arise in captivity, but there have also been examples in the wild. Hybridization occurs where two species' range overlap to form hybrid zones; hybrids may be created by humans when animals are placed in zoos or due to environmental pressures such as predation. Intergeneric hybridizations, hybrids of different genera, have also been found in the wild. Although they belong to genera that have been distinct for several million years, interbreeding still occurs between the gelada and the hamadryas baboon.

Clones

On 24 January 2018, scientists in China reported in the journal Cell the creation of two crab-eating macaque clones, named Zhong Zhong and Hua Hua, using the complex DNA transfer method that produced Dolly the sheep, for the first time.

Anatomy and physiology

image
Primate skulls showing postorbital bar, and increasing brain sizes

The primate skull has a large, domed cranium, which is particularly prominent in anthropoids. The cranium protects the large brain, a distinguishing characteristic of this group. The endocranial volume (the volume within the skull) is three times greater in humans than in the greatest nonhuman primate, reflecting a larger brain size. The mean endocranial volume is 1,201 cubic centimeters in humans, 469 cm3 in gorillas, 400 cm3 in chimpanzees and 397 cm3 in orangutans. The primary evolutionary trend of primates has been the elaboration of the brain, in particular the neocortex (a part of the cerebral cortex), which is involved with sensory perception, generation of motor commands, spatial reasoning, conscious thought and, in humans, language. While other mammals rely heavily on their sense of smell, the arboreal life of primates has led to a tactile, visually dominant sensory system, a reduction in the olfactory region of the brain and increasingly complex social behavior. The visual acuity of humans and other hominids is exceptional; they have the most acute vision known among all vertebrates, with the exception of certain species of predatory birds.

Primates have forward-facing eyes on the front of the skull; binocular vision allows accurate distance perception, useful for the brachiating ancestors of all great apes. A bony ridge above the eye sockets reinforces weaker bones in the face, which are put under strain during chewing. Strepsirrhines have a postorbital bar, a bone around the eye socket, to protect their eyes; in contrast, the higher primates, haplorhines, have evolved fully enclosed sockets.

image
An 1893 drawing of the hands and feet of various primates

Primates show an evolutionary trend towards a reduced snout. Technically, Old World monkeys are distinguished from New World monkeys by the structure of the nose, and from apes by the arrangement of their teeth. In New World monkeys, the nostrils face sideways; in Old World monkeys, they face downwards. Dental pattern in primates vary considerably; although some have lost most of their incisors, all retain at least one lower incisor. In most strepsirrhines, the lower incisors form a toothcomb, which is used in grooming and sometimes foraging. Old World monkeys have eight premolars, compared with 12 in New World monkeys. The Old World species are divided into apes and monkeys depending on the number of cusps on their molars: monkeys have four, apes have five - although humans may have four or five. The main hominid molar cusp (hypocone) evolved in early primate history, while the cusp of the corresponding primitive lower molar (paraconid) was lost. Prosimians are distinguished by their immobilized upper lips, the moist tip of their noses and forward-facing lower front teeth.

Body

image
Vervet hindfoot showing fingerprint ridges on the sole

Primates generally have five digits on each limb (pentadactyly), with a characteristic type of keratin fingernail on the end of each finger and toe. The bottom sides of the hands and feet have sensitive pads on the fingertips. Most have opposable thumbs, a characteristic primate feature most developed in humans, though not limited to this order (opossums and koalas, for example, also have them). Thumbs allow some species to use tools. In primates, the combination of opposing thumbs, short fingernails (rather than claws) and long, inward-closing fingers is a relict of the ancestral practice of gripping branches, and has, in part, allowed some species to develop brachiation (swinging by the arms from tree limb to tree limb) as a significant means of locomotion. Prosimians have clawlike nails on the second toe of each foot, called toilet-claws, which they use for grooming.

The primate collar bone is a prominent element of the pectoral girdle; this allows the shoulder joint broad mobility. Compared to Old World monkeys, apes have more mobile shoulder joints and arms due to the dorsal position of the scapula, broad ribcages that are flatter front-to-back, a shorter, less mobile spine, and with lower vertebrae greatly reduced - resulting in tail loss in some species.Prehensile tails are found in the New World atelids, including the howler, spider, woolly spider, woolly monkeys; and in capuchins. Male primates have a low-hanging penis and testes descended into a scrotum.

Sexual dimorphism

image
Distinct sexual size dimorphism can be seen between the male and female gorilla.

Sexual dimorphism is often exhibited in simians, though to a greater degree in Old World species (apes and some monkeys) than New World species. Recent studies involve comparing DNA to examine both the variation in the expression of the dimorphism among primates and the fundamental causes of sexual dimorphism. Primates usually have dimorphism in body mass and canine tooth size along with pelage and skin color. The dimorphism can be attributed to and affected by different factors, including mating system, size, habitat and diet.

Comparative analyses have generated a more complete understanding of the relationship between sexual selection, natural selection, and mating systems in primates. Studies have shown that dimorphism is the product of changes in both male and female traits. Ontogenetic scaling, where relative extension of a common growth trajectory occurs, may give some insight into the relationship between sexual dimorphism and growth patterns. Some evidence from the fossil record suggests that there was convergent evolution of dimorphism, and some extinct hominids probably had greater dimorphism than any living primate.

Locomotion

image
Diademed sifaka, a lemur that is a vertical clinger and leaper

Primate species move by brachiation, bipedalism, leaping, arboreal and terrestrial quadrupedalism, climbing, knuckle-walking or by a combination of these methods. Several prosimians are primarily vertical clingers and leapers. These include many bushbabies, all indriids (i.e., sifakas, avahis and indris), sportive lemurs, and all tarsiers. Other prosimians are arboreal quadrupeds and climbers. Some are also terrestrial quadrupeds, while some are leapers. Most monkeys are both arboreal and terrestrial quadrupeds and climbers. Gibbons, muriquis and spider monkeys all brachiate extensively, with gibbons sometimes doing so in remarkably acrobatic fashion. Woolly monkeys also brachiate at times.Orangutans use a similar form of locomotion called quadramanous climbing, in which they use their arms and legs to carry their heavy bodies through the trees.Chimpanzees and gorillas knuckle walk, and can move bipedally for short distances. Although numerous species, such as australopithecines and early hominids, have exhibited fully bipedal locomotion, humans are the only extant species with this trait.

Vision

image
The tapetum lucidum of a northern greater galago, typical of prosimians, reflects the light of the photographer's flash.

The evolution of color vision in primates is unique among most eutherian mammals. While the remote vertebrate ancestors of the primates possessed three color vision (trichromaticism), the nocturnal, warm-blooded, mammalian ancestors lost one of three cones in the retina during the Mesozoic era. Fish, reptiles and birds are therefore trichromatic or tetrachromatic, while all mammals, with the exception of some primates and marsupials, are dichromats or monochromats (totally color blind). Nocturnal primates, such as the night monkeys and bush babies, are often monochromatic. Catarrhines are routinely trichromatic due to a gene duplication of the red-green opsin gene at the base of their lineage, 30 to 40 million years ago. Platyrrhines, on the other hand, are trichromatic in a few cases only. Specifically, individual females must be heterozygous for two alleles of the opsin gene (red and green) located on the same locus of the X chromosome. Males, therefore, can only be dichromatic, while females can be either dichromatic or trichromatic. Color vision in strepsirrhines is not as well understood; however, research indicates a range of color vision similar to that found in platyrrhines.

Like catarrhines, howler monkeys (a family of platyrrhines) show routine trichromatism that has been traced to an evolutionarily recent gene duplication. Howler monkeys are one of the most specialized leaf-eaters of the New World monkeys; fruits are not a major part of their diets, and the type of leaves they prefer to consume (young, nutritive, and digestible) are detectable only by a red-green signal. Field work exploring the dietary preferences of howler monkeys suggests that routine trichromaticism was selected by environment.

Behavior

Social systems

Richard Wrangham stated that social systems of primates are best classified by the amount of movement by females occurring between groups. He proposed four categories:

  • Female transfer systems – females move away from the group in which they were born. Females of a group will not be closely related whereas males will have remained with their natal groups, and this close association may be influential in social behavior. The groups formed are generally quite small. This organization can be seen in chimpanzees, where the males, who are typically related, will cooperate in defense of the group's territory. Evidence of this social system has also been found among Neanderthal remains in Spain and in remains of Australopithecus and Paranthropus robustus groups in southern Africa. Among New World Monkeys, spider monkeys and muriquis use this system.
image
A social huddle of ring-tailed lemurs. The two individuals on the right exposing their white ventral surface are sunning themselves.
  • Male transfer systems – while the females remain in their natal groups, the males will emigrate as adolescents. Group sizes are usually larger. This system is common among the ring-tailed lemur, capuchin monkeys and cercopithecine monkeys.
  • Monogamous species – a male–female bond, sometimes accompanied by a juvenile offspring. There is shared responsibility of parental care and territorial defense. The offspring leaves the parents' territory during adolescence.Indri, lariang tarsiers, Callitrichidae monkeys and gibbons use this system, although "monogamy" in this context does not necessarily mean absolute sexual fidelity. These species do not live in larger groups.
  • Solitary species – males and females live in overlapping home ranges. This type of organization is found in lorises, galagos, mouse lemurs, aye-ayes and orangutans.

Other systems are known to occur as well. For example, with howler monkeys and gorillas both the males and females typically transfer from their natal group on reaching sexual maturity, resulting in groups in which neither the males nor females are typically related. Some prosimians, colobine monkeys and callitrichid monkeys also use this system.

The transfer of females or males from their native group is likely an adaptation for avoiding inbreeding. An analysis of breeding records of captive primate colonies representing numerous different species indicates that the infant mortality of inbred young is generally higher than that of non-inbred young. This effect of inbreeding on infant mortality is probably largely a result of increased expression of deleterious recessive alleles (see Inbreeding depression).

image
Chimpanzees are social great apes.

Primatologist Jane Goodall, who studied in the Gombe Stream National Park, noted fission-fusion societies in chimpanzees. There is fission when the main group splits up to forage during the day, then fusion when the group returns at night to sleep as a group. This social structure can also be observed in the hamadryas baboon,spider monkeys and the bonobo. The gelada has a similar social structure in which many smaller groups come together to form temporary herds of up to 600 monkeys.Humans also form fission-fusion societies. In hunter-gatherer societies, humans form groups which are made up of several individuals that may split up to obtain different resources.

These social systems are affected by three main ecological factors: distribution of resources, group size, and predation. Within a social group there is a balance between cooperation and competition. Cooperative behaviors in many primates species include social grooming (removing skin parasites and cleaning wounds), food sharing, and collective defense against predators or of a territory. Aggressive behaviors often signal competition for food, sleeping sites or mates. Aggression is also used in establishing dominance hierarchies.

In November 2023, scientists reported, for the first time, evidence that groups of primates, particularly bonobos, are capable of cooperating with each other.

Interspecific associations

Several species of primates are known to associate in the wild. Some of these associations have been extensively studied. In the Tai Forest of Africa, several species coordinate anti-predator behavior. These include the Diana monkey, Campbell's mona monkey, lesser spot-nosed monkey, western red colobus, king colobus (western black and white colobus), and sooty mangabey, which coordinate anti-predator alarm calls. Among the predators of these monkeys is the common chimpanzee.

The red-tailed monkey associates with several species, including the western red colobus, blue monkey, Wolf's mona monkey, mantled guereza, black crested mangabey and Allen's swamp monkey. Several of these species are preyed upon by the common chimpanzee.

In South America, squirrel monkeys associate with capuchin monkeys. This may have more to do with foraging benefits to the squirrel monkeys than anti-predation benefits.

Mating systems

image
Two common marmosets, the species lives in monogamous pairs
image
Gelada harem: one male and multiple females

The mating systems of primates vary between monogamy, polyandry, polygyny and polygynandry. In monogamous species, adult males and females form long-lasting pair bonds. Compared to other systems, there is little competition for mating rights and males and females tend to be similar in size. Polyandry, which involves groups consisting of single females mating with multiple males, may arise as a secondary mating system in monogamous species. In the brown-mantled tamarin, a female may breeding with one or two males. Polyandry may have developed due to the high frequency of twin births, which require more help in raising.

Polygynous species include gorillas, Hanuman langurs, geladas, hamadryas baboons, proboscis monkeys, and golden snub-nosed monkeys, and consists of one male mating with multiple females within a harem or one-male unit. Sexual dimorphism tends to be higher in these species and males may also develop prominent secondary sex characteristics. In the patriarchal hamadryas baboon, the males aggressively herd females into their groups and violently discipline those that wander. By contrast, in gelada society, which is based on female kinship, a male is dependent on the support of the females in his unit and cannot impose on them. Polygynous males must defend their harems from rivals, who may try to take over.

In some species, such as ring-tailed lemurs, sifakas, macaques, most baboons, mangabeys, squirrel monkeys, woolly monkeys, spider monkeys, woolly spider monkeys, chimpanzees and bonobos, both males and females mate with multiple partners. Polygynandry occurs in multimale-multifemale groups, and since females mate many times before conception, males have large testicles for sperm competition. Males may exist in a dominance hierarchy and those at the top will try to monopolize access to the females. Consortships may occur in some species but these are short-term. In solitary-living species, males and females mate with partners whose home ranges they overlap with. This is known as a 'dispersed' mating system.

Genetic evidence indicates that humans were predominantly polygynous for most of their existence as a species, but that this began to shift during the Neolithic, when monogamy started becoming widespread concomitantly with the transition from nomadic to sedentary societies. Most modern human societies consist of monogamous marriages, but allow for polygyny, particularly for those of a high status.

Sexual behavior

image
Bonobos mating, Jacksonville Zoo and Gardens.

Female primates may signal to the male their receptiveness though various displays including eye-contact, tongue-clicking and presenting of the rump. Female lemurs, lorises and galagos will position themselves in the lordosis pose while female chimpanzees, bonobos and some Old World monkeys develop sexual swellings on the rump. Copulation in primates typically involves the males mounting the females from behind, as with most mammals. Belly-to-belly copulation has been recorded in apes, both gibbons and the great apes. Human sex positions are modifications of these two positions.

Primates may egage in sexual activity as part of social bonding; including homosexual behaviour. Such behavior play an important role in bonobo society in particular. female bonobos engage in mutual genital-rubbing behavior, possibly to bond socially with each other, thus forming a female nucleus of bonobo society. The bonding among females enables them to dominate most of the males.

Life history

image
A crab-eating macaque breastfeeding her baby

Primates have slower rates of development than other mammals. All primate infants are breastfed by their mothers (with the exception of some human cultures and various zoo raised primates which are fed formula) and rely on them for grooming and transportation. In some species, infants are protected and transported by males in the group, particularly males who may be their fathers. Other relatives of the infant, such as siblings and aunts, may participate in its care as well. Most primate mothers cease ovulation while breastfeeding an infant; once the infant is weaned the mother can reproduce again. This often leads to weaning conflict with infants who attempt to continue breastfeeding.

Infanticide is common in polygynous species such as gray langurs and gorillas. Adult males may kill dependent offspring that are not theirs so the female will return to estrus and thus they can sire offspring of their own. Social monogamy in some species may have evolved to combat this behavior. Polygynandry may also lessen the risk of infanticide since paternity becomes uncertain.

Primates have a longer juvenile period between weaning and sexual maturity than other mammals of similar size. Some primates such as galagos and New World monkeys use tree-holes for nesting, and park juveniles in leafy patches while foraging. Other primates follow a strategy of "riding", i.e. carrying individuals on the body while feeding. Adults may construct or use nesting sites, sometimes accompanied by juveniles, for the purpose of resting, a behavior which has developed secondarily in the great apes. During the juvenile period, primates are more susceptible than adults to predation and starvation; they gain experience in feeding and avoiding predators during this time. They learn social and fighting skills, often through playing. Primates, especially females, have longer lifespans than other similarly sized mammals, this may be partially due to their slower metabolisms. Late in life, female catarrhine primates appear to undergo a cessation of reproductive function known as menopause; other groups are less studied.

Diet and feeding

image
Leaf eating mantled guereza
image
A mouse lemur eating fruit

Primates exploit a variety of food sources. It has been said that many characteristics of modern primates, including humans, derive from an early ancestor's practice of taking most of its food from the tropical canopy. Most primates include fruit in their diets to obtain easily digested nutrients including carbohydrates and lipids for energy. Primates in the suborder Strepsirrhini (non-tarsier prosimians) are able to synthesize vitamin C, like most other mammals, while primates of the suborder Haplorhini (tarsiers, monkeys and apes) have lost this ability, and require the vitamin in their diet.

Many primates have anatomical specializations that enable them to exploit particular foods, such as fruit, leaves, gum or insects. For example, leaf eaters such as howler monkeys, black-and-white colobuses and sportive lemurs have extended digestive tracts which enable them to absorb nutrients from leaves that can be difficult to digest.Marmosets, which are gum eaters, have strong incisor teeth, enabling them to open tree bark to get to the gum, and claws rather than nails, enabling them to cling to trees while feeding. The aye-aye combines rodent-like teeth with a long, thin middle finger to fill the same ecological niche as a woodpecker. It taps on trees to find insect larvae, then gnaws holes in the wood and inserts its elongated middle finger to pull the larvae out. Some species have additional specializations. For example, the grey-cheeked mangabey has thick enamel on its teeth, enabling it to open hard fruits and seeds that other monkeys cannot. The gelada is the only primate species that feeds primarily on grass.

Hunting

image
Humans have traditionally hunted prey for subsistence.

Tarsiers are the only extant obligate carnivorous primates, exclusively eating insects, crustaceans, small vertebrates and snakes (including venomous species).Capuchin monkeys can exploit many different types of plant matter, including fruit, leaves, flowers, buds, nectar and seeds, but also eat insects and other invertebrates, bird eggs, and small vertebrates such as birds, lizards, squirrels and bats.

The common chimpanzee eats an omnivorous frugivorous diet. It prefers fruit above all other food items and even seeks out and eats them when they are not abundant. It also eats leaves and leaf buds, seeds, blossoms, stems, pith, bark and resin. Insects and meat make up a small proportion of their diet, estimated as 2%. The meat consumption includes predation on other primate species, such as the western red colobus monkey. The bonobo is an omnivorous frugivore – the majority of its diet is fruit, but it supplements this with leaves, meat from small vertebrates, such as anomalures, flying squirrels and duikers, and invertebrates. In some instances, bonobos have been shown to consume lower-order primates.

Until the development of agriculture approximately 10,000 years ago, Homo sapiens employed a hunter-gatherer method as their sole means of food collection. This involved combining stationary food sources (such as fruits, grains, tubers, and mushrooms, insect larvae and aquatic mollusks) with wild game, which must be hunted and killed in order to be consumed. It has been proposed that humans have used fire to prepare and cook food since the time of Homo erectus. Around ten thousand years ago, humans developed agriculture, which substantially altered their diet. This change in diet may also have altered human biology; with the spread of dairy farming providing a new and rich source of food, leading to the evolution of the ability to digest lactose in some adults.

As prey

Predators of primates include various species of carnivorans, birds of prey, reptiles, and other primates. Even gorillas have been recorded as prey. Predators of primates have diverse hunting strategies and as such, primates have evolved several different antipredator adaptations including crypsis, alarm calls and mobbing. Several species have separate alarm calls for different predators such as air-borne or ground-dwelling predators. Predation may have shaped group size in primates as species exposed to higher predation pressures appear to live in larger groups.

Communication

image
A pair of black howler monkeys vocalizing

Lemurs, lorises, tarsiers, and New World monkeys rely on olfactory signals for many aspects of social and reproductive behavior. Specialized glands are used to mark territories with pheromones, which are detected by the vomeronasal organ; this process forms a large part of the communication behavior of these primates. In Old World monkeys and apes this ability is mostly vestigial, having regressed as trichromatic eyes evolved to become the main sensory organ. Primates also use vocalizations, gestures, and facial expressions to convey psychological state. Facial musculature is very developed in primates, particularly in monkeys and apes, allowing for complex facial communication. Like humans, chimpanzees can distinguish the faces of familiar and unfamiliar individuals. Hand and arm gestures are also important forms of communication for great apes and a single gesture can have multiple functions. Chest-beating in male gorillas is a form of visual and non-vocal sound communication that serves to show fitness to both rivals and females.

Primates are a particularly vocal group of mammals.Indris and black-and-white ruffed lemurs make distinctive, loud songs and choruses which maintain territories and act as alarm calls. The Philippine tarsier, has a high-frequency limit of auditory sensitivity of approximately 91 kHz with a dominant frequency of 70 kHz, among the highest recorded for any terrestrial mammal. For Philippine tarsiers, these ultrasonic vocalizations might represent a private channel of communication that subverts detection by predators, prey and competitors, enhances energetic efficiency, or improves detection against low-frequency background noise. Male howler monkeys are among the loudest land mammals as their roars can be heard up to 4.8 km (3.0 mi), and relate to intergroup spacing, territorial protection and possibly mate-guarding. Male and female siamangs both possess inflatable pouches in the throat with which pair -bonds use to sing "duets" to each other. The vervet monkey gives a distinct alarm call for each of at least four different predators, and the reactions of other monkeys vary according to the call. Furthermore, many primate species including chimpanzees,Campbell's mona monkeys or Diana monkeys have been shown to combine vocalizations in sequences, suggesting syntax may not be uniquely humans as previously thought but rather evolutionary ancient, and its origins may be deeply rooted in the primate lineage.

Consonant- and vowel-like sounds exist in some orangutan calls and they maintain their meaning over great distances. The time range for the evolution of human language and/or its anatomical prerequisites extends, at least in principle, from the phylogenetic divergence of Homo (2.3 to 2.4 million years ago) from Pan (5 to 6 million years ago) to the emergence of full behavioral modernity some 50,000–150,000 years ago. Few dispute that Australopithecus probably lacked vocal communication significantly more sophisticated than that of great apes in general.

Intelligence and cognition

Primates have advanced cognitive abilities: some make tools and use them to acquire food and for social displays; some can perform tasks requiring cooperation, influence and rank; they are status conscious, manipulative and capable of deception; they can recognise kin and conspecifics; and they can learn to use symbols and understand aspects of human language including some relational syntax and concepts of number and numerical sequence. Research in primate cognition explores problem solving, memory, social interaction, a theory of mind, and numerical, spatial, and abstract concepts. Comparative studies show a trend towards higher intelligence going from prosimians to New World monkeys to Old World monkeys, and significantly higher average cognitive abilities in the great apes. However, there is a great deal of variation in each group (e.g., among New World monkeys, both spider and capuchin monkeys have scored highly by some measures), as well as in the results of different studies.

Tool use and manufacture

Chimpanzees using twigs to dip for ants
image
Crab-eating macaques with stone tools

In 1960, Jane Goodall observed a chimpanzee poking pieces of grass into a termite mound and then raising the grass to his mouth. After he left, Goodall approached the mound and repeated the behaviour because she was unsure what the chimpanzee was doing. She found that the termites bit onto the grass with their jaws. The chimpanzee had been using the grass as a tool to "fish" or "dip" for termites. There are more limited reports of the closely related bonobo using tools in the wild; it has been claimed they rarely use tools in the wild although they use tools as readily as chimpanzees when in captivity. It has been reported that females, both chimpanzee and bonobo, use tools more avidly than males.Orangutans in Borneo scoop catfish out of small ponds. Over two years, anthropologist Anne Russon observed orangutans learning to jab sticks at catfish to scare them out of the ponds and in to their waiting hands. There are few reports of gorillas using tools in the wild. An adult female western lowland gorilla used a branch as a walking stick apparently to test water depth and to aid her in crossing a pool of water. Another adult female used a detached trunk from a small shrub as a stabilizer during food gathering, and another used a log as a bridge.

The first direct observation of a non-ape primate using a tool in a wild environment occurred in 1988. Primatologist Sue Boinski watched an adult male white-faced capuchin beat a fer-de-lance snake to death with a dead branch. The black-striped capuchin was the first non-ape primate for which routine tool use was documented in the wild; individuals were observed cracking nuts by placing them on a stone anvil and hitting them with another large stone. In Thailand and Myanmar, crab-eating macaques use stone tools to open nuts, oysters and other bivalves, and various types of sea snails. Chacma baboons use stones as weapons; stoning by these baboons is done from the rocky walls of the canyon where they sleep and retreat to when they are threatened. Stones are lifted with one hand and dropped over the side whereupon they tumble down the side of the cliff or fall directly to the canyon floor.

Although they have not been observed to use tools in the wild, lemurs in controlled settings have been shown to be capable of understanding the functional properties of the objects they had been trained to use as tools, performing as well as tool-using haplorhines.

Soon after her initial discovery of tool use, Goodall observed other chimpanzees picking up leafy twigs, stripping off the leaves and using the stems to fish for insects. This change of a leafy twig into a tool was a major discovery. Prior to this, scientists thought that only humans manufactured and used tools, and that this ability was what separated humans from other animals. Chimpanzees have also been observed making "sponges" out of leaves and moss that suck up water. Sumatran orangutans have been observed making and using tools. They will break off a tree branch that is about 30 cm long, snap off the twigs, fray one end and then use the stick to dig in tree holes for termites. In the wild, mandrills have been observed to clean their ears with modified tools. Scientists filmed a large male mandrill at Chester Zoo (UK) stripping down a twig, apparently to make it narrower, and then using the modified stick to scrape dirt from underneath its toenails. Captive gorillas have made a variety of tools.

Ecology

image
Rhesus macaque at Agra Fort, India

Non-human primates primarily live in the tropical latitudes of Africa, Asia, and the Americas. Species that live outside of the tropics include the Japanese macaque which lives in the Japanese islands of Honshū and Hokkaido; the Barbary macaque which lives in North Africa and several species of langur which live in China. Primates tend to live in tropical rainforests but are also found in temperate forests, savannas, deserts, mountains and coastal areas. The number of primate species within tropical areas has been shown to be positively correlated to the amount of rainfall and the amount of rain forest area. Accounting for 25% to 40% of the fruit-eating animals (by weight) within tropical rainforests, primates play an important ecological role by dispersing seeds of many tree species.

Primate habitats span a range of altitudes: the black snub-nosed monkey has been found living in the Hengduan Mountains at altitudes of 4,700 meters (15,400 ft), the mountain gorilla can be found at 4,200 meters (13,200 ft) crossing the Virunga Mountains, and the gelada has been found at elevations of up to 5,000 m (16,000 ft) in the Ethiopian Highlands. Some species interact with aquatic environments and may swim or even dive, including the proboscis monkey, De Brazza's monkey and Allen's swamp monkey. Some primates, such as the rhesus macaque and gray langurs, can exploit human-modified environments and even live in cities.

Interactions between humans and other primates

Disease transmission

Close interactions between humans and non-human primates (NHPs) can create pathways for the transmission of zoonotic diseases. Viruses such as Herpesviridae (most notably Herpes B Virus), Poxviridae, measles, ebola, rabies, the Marburg virus and viral hepatitis can be transmitted to humans; in some cases the viruses produce potentially fatal diseases in both humans and non-human primates.

image
Slow lorises are popular in the exotic pet trade, which threatens wild populations.

Only humans are recognized as persons and protected in law by the United Nations Universal Declaration of Human Rights. The legal status of NHPs, on the other hand, is the subject of much debate, with organizations such as the Great Ape Project (GAP) campaigning to award at least some of them legal rights. In June 2008, Spain became the first country in the world to recognize the rights of some NHPs, when its parliament's cross-party environmental committee urged the country to comply with GAP's recommendations, which are that chimpanzees, orangutans and gorillas are not to be used for animal experiments.

Many species of NHP are kept as pets by humans. The Allied Effort to Save Other Primates (AESOP) estimates that around 15,000 NHPs live as exotic pets in the United States. The expanding Chinese middle class has increased demand for NHPs as exotic pets in recent years. Although NHP import for the pet trade was banned in the U.S. in 1975, smuggling still occurs along the United States – Mexico border, with prices ranging from US$3000 for monkeys to $30,000 for apes.

Primates are used as model organisms in laboratories and have been used in space missions. They serve as service animals for disabled humans. Capuchin monkeys can be trained to assist quadriplegic humans; their intelligence, memory, and manual dexterity make them ideal helpers.

NHPs are kept in zoos around the globe. Historically, zoos were primarily a form of entertainment, but more recently have shifted their focus towards conservation, education and research. GAP does not insist that all NHPs should be released from zoos, primarily because captive-born primates lack the knowledge and experience to survive in the wild if released.

Role in scientific research

image
Sam, a rhesus macaque, was flown to the edge of space by NASA in the 1959 Little Joe 2 flight of Project Mercury.

Thousands of non-human primates are used around the world in research because of their psychological and physiological similarity to humans. In particular, the brains and eyes of NHPs more closely parallel human anatomy than those of any other animals. NHPs are commonly used in preclinical trials, neuroscience, ophthalmology studies, and toxicity studies. Rhesus macaques are often used, as are other macaques, African green monkeys, chimpanzees, baboons, squirrel monkeys, and marmosets, both wild-caught and purpose-bred.

In 2005, GAP reported that 1,280 of the 3,100 NHPs living in captivity in the United States were used for experiments. In 2004, the European Union used around 10,000 NHPs in such experiments; in 2005 in Great Britain, 4,652 experiments were conducted on 3,115 NHPs. Governments of many nations have strict care requirements of NHPs kept in captivity. In the US, federal guidelines extensively regulate aspects of NHP housing, feeding, enrichment, and breeding. European groups such as the European Coalition to End Animal Experiments are seeking a ban on all NHP use in experiments as part of the European Union's review of animal testing legislation.

Extinction threats

image
Humans are known to hunt other primates for food, called bushmeat. Pictured are two men who have killed a number of silky sifaka and white-headed brown lemurs.

The International Union for Conservation of Nature (IUCN) lists more than a third of primates as critically endangered or vulnerable. About 60% of primate species are threatened with extinction, including: 87% of species in Madagascar, 73% in Asia, 37% in Africa, and 36% in South and Central America. Additionally, 75% of primate species have decreasing populations. Trade is regulated, as all species are listed by CITES in Appendix II, except 50 species and subspecies listed in Appendix I, which gain full protection from trade.

Common threats to primate species include deforestation, forest fragmentation, monkey drives (resulting from primate crop raiding), and primate hunting for use in medicines, as pets, and for food. Large-scale tropical forest clearing is widely regarded as the process that most threatens primates. More than 90% of primate species occur in tropical forests. The main cause of forest loss is clearing for agriculture, although commercial logging, subsistence harvesting of timber, mining, and dam construction also contribute to tropical forest destruction. In Indonesia large areas of lowland forest have been cleared to increase palm oil production, and one analysis of satellite imagery concluded that during 1998 and 1999 there was a loss of 1,000 Sumatran orangutans per year in the Leuser Ecosystem alone.

image
The critically endangered silky sifaka

Primates with a large body size (over 5 kg) are at increased extinction risk due to their greater profitability to poachers compared to smaller primates. They reach sexual maturity later and have a longer period between births. Populations therefore recover more slowly after being depleted by poaching or the pet trade. Data for some African cities show that half of all protein consumed in urban areas comes from the bushmeat trade. Endangered primates such as guenons and the drill are hunted at levels that far exceed sustainable levels. This is due to their large body size, ease of transport and profitability per animal. As farming encroaches on forest habitats, primates feed on the crops, causing the farmers large economic losses. Primate crop raiding gives locals a negative impression of primates, hindering conservation efforts.

Madagascar, home to five endemic primate families, has experienced the greatest extinction of the recent past; since human settlement 1,500 years ago, at least eight classes and fifteen of the larger species have become extinct due to hunting and habitat destruction. Among the primates wiped out were Archaeoindris (a lemur larger than a silverback gorilla) and the families Palaeopropithecidae and Archaeolemuridae.

image
The critically endangered Sumatran orangutan

In Asia, Hinduism, Buddhism, and Islam prohibit eating primate meat; however, primates are still hunted for food. Some smaller traditional religions allow the consumption of primate meat. The pet trade and traditional medicine also increase demand for illegal hunting. The rhesus macaque, a model organism, was protected after excessive trapping threatened its numbers in the 1960s; the program was so effective that they are now viewed as a pest throughout their range.

In Central and South America, forest fragmentation and hunting are the two main problems for primates. Large tracts of forest are now rare in Central America. This increases the amount of forest vulnerable to edge effects such as farmland encroachment, lower levels of humidity and a change in plant life. Movement restriction results in a greater amount of inbreeding, which can cause deleterious effects leading to a population bottleneck, whereby a significant percentage of the population is lost.

There are 21 critically endangered primates, seven of which have remained on the IUCN's "The World's 25 Most Endangered Primates" list since the year 2000: the silky sifaka, Delacour's langur, the white-headed langur, the gray-shanked douc, the Tonkin snub-nosed monkey, the Cross River gorilla and the Sumatran orangutan.Miss Waldron's red colobus was recently declared extinct when no trace of the subspecies could be found from 1993 to 1999. A few hunters have found and killed individuals since then, but the subspecies' prospects remain bleak.

See also

  • Arboreal theory
  • Great Ape Project
  • Human evolution
  • International Primate Day
  • List of primates
  • List of fossil primates
  • Monkey Day
  • Primatology

Footnotes

  1. Although the monophyletic relationship between lemurs and lorisoids is widely accepted, their clade name is not. The term "lemuriform" is used here because it derives from one popular taxonomy that clumps the clade of toothcombed primates into one infraorder and the extinct, non-toothcombed adapiforms into another, both within the suborder Strepsirrhini. However, another popular alternative taxonomy places the lorisoids in their own infraorder, Lorisiformes.
  2. Article 6: Everyone has the right to recognition everywhere as a person before the law.

References

  1. Groves, C. P. (2005). Wilson, D. E.; Reeder, D. M. (eds.). Mammal Species of the World: A Taxonomic and Geographic Reference (3rd ed.). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press. pp. 111–184. ISBN 0-801-88221-4. OCLC 62265494.
  2. Silcox, Mary T.; Bloch, Jonathan I.; Boyer, Doug M.; Chester, Stephen G. B.; López-Torres, Sergi (2017). "The evolutionary radiation of plesiadapiforms". Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews. 26 (2): 74–94. doi:10.1002/evan.21526. ISSN 1520-6505. PMID 28429568.
  3. "Primate". Merriam-Webster Online Dictionary. Merriam-Webster. Retrieved 2008-07-21.
  4. The Book of Popular Science. 1963. p. 257.
  5. Chisholm, Hugh, ed. (1911). "Ape" . Encyclopædia Britannica. Vol. 02 (11th ed.). Cambridge University Press. p. 160.
  6. Weisberger, Mindy (March 23, 2024). "Why don't humans have tails? Scientists find answers in an unlikely place". CNN. Archived from the original on March 24, 2024. Retrieved March 24, 2024.
  7. Dixson, A.F. (1981), The Natural History of the Gorilla, London: Weidenfeld & Nicolson, ISBN 978-0-297-77895-0
  8. Definitions of paraphyly vary; for the one used here see e.g. Stace, Clive A. (2010), "Classification by molecules: What's in it for field botanists?" (PDF), Watsonia, 28: 103–122, archived from the original (PDF) on 2011-07-26, retrieved 2010-02-07.
  9. Definitions of monophyly vary; for the one used here see e.g. Mishler, Brent D (2009), "Species are not Uniquely Real Biological Entities", in Ayala, F.J. & Arp, R. (eds.), Contemporary Debates in Philosophy of Biology, pp. 110–122, doi:10.1002/9781444314922.ch6, ISBN 978-1-4443-1492-2.
  10. Benton 2005, p. 371.
  11. Benton 2005, pp. 378–380.
  12. Linnaeus, C. (1758). Sistema naturae per regna tria Naturae, secundum classes, ordines, genera, species, cum characteribus differentiis, synonimis locis. Tomus I. Impensis direct. Laurentii Salvii, Holmia. pp. 20–32.
  13. Linnaeus, C. (1735). Sistema naturae sive regna tria Naturae systematice proposita per classes, ordines, genera, & species. apud Theodorum Haak, Lugduni Batavorum. pp. s.p.
  14. Blainville, H. (1839). "Nouvelle classification des Mammifères". Annales Françaises et Etrangères d'Anatomie et de Physiologie Appliquées à la Médicine et à l'Histoire Naturelle, 3. pp. 268–269.
  15. Thorington, R. W. & Anderson, S. (1984). "Primates". In Anderson, S. & Jones, J. K. (eds.). Orders and Families of Recent Mammals of the World. New York: John Wiley and Sons. pp. 187–217. ISBN 978-0-471-08493-8.
  16. McKenna, M. C. & Bell, S. K. (1997). Classification of Mammals: Above the species level. New York: Columbia University Press. p. 631. ISBN 0-231-11013-8.
  17. Strier, K. (2007). Primate Behavioral Ecology (Third ed.). Pearson Allyn and Bacon. pp. 50–53. ISBN 978-0-205-44432-8.
  18. Cartmill, M.; Smith, F. H. (2011). The Human Lineage. John Wiley & Sons. ISBN 978-1-118-21145-8.
  19. Groves, C. P. (2001). Primate Taxonomy. Smithsonian Institution Press. ISBN 1-56098-872-X.
  20. Szalay & Delson 1980, p. 149.
  21. Cartmill 2010, p. 15.
  22. Hartwig 2011, pp. 20–21.
  23. Janečka, J. E.; Miller, W.; Pringle, T. H.; Wiens, F.; Zitzmann, A.; Helgen, K. M.; Springer, M. S.; Murphy, W. J. (2 November 2007). "Molecular and Genomic Data Identify the Closest Living Relative of Primates". Science. 318 (5851): 792–794. Bibcode:2007Sci...318..792J. doi:10.1126/science.1147555. PMID 17975064. S2CID 12251814.
  24. Kavanagh, M. (1983). A Complete Guide to Monkeys, Apes and Other Primates. New York: Viking Press. pp. 18. ISBN 0-670-43543-0.
  25. McKenna, M. C. & Bell, S. K. (1997). Classification of Mammals Above the Species Level. New York: Columbia University Press. p. 329. ISBN 0-231-11012-X.
  26. Williams, B.A.; Kay, R.F.; Kirk, E.C. (2010). "New perspectives on anthropoid origins". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 107 (11): 4797–4804. Bibcode:2010PNAS..107.4797W. doi:10.1073/pnas.0908320107. PMC 2841917. PMID 20212104.
  27. Stanyon, Roscoe; Springer, Mark S.; Meredith, Robert W.; Gatesy, John; Emerling, Christopher A.; Park, Jong; Rabosky, Daniel L.; Stadler, Tanja; Steiner, Cynthia; Ryder, Oliver A.; Janečka, Jan E.; Fisher, Colleen A.; Murphy, William J. (2012). "Macroevolutionary Dynamics and Historical Biogeography of Primate Diversification Inferred from a Species Supermatrix". PLOS ONE. 7 (11): e49521. Bibcode:2012PLoSO...749521S. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0049521. ISSN 1932-6203. PMC 3500307. PMID 23166696.
  28. Jameson, Natalie M.; Hou, Zhuo-Cheng; Sterner, Kirstin N.; Weckle, Amy; Goodman, Morris; Steiper, Michael E.; Wildman, Derek E. (September 2011). "Genomic data reject the hypothesis of a prosimian primate clade". Journal of Human Evolution. 61 (3): 295–305. Bibcode:2011JHumE..61..295J. doi:10.1016/j.jhevol.2011.04.004. ISSN 0047-2484. PMID 21620437.
  29. Pozzi, Luca; Hodgson, Jason A.; Burrell, Andrew S.; Sterner, Kirstin N.; Raaum, Ryan L.; Disotell, Todd R. (June 2014). "Primate phylogenetic relationships and divergence dates inferred from complete mitochondrial genomes". Molecular Phylogenetics and Evolution. 75: 165–183. Bibcode:2014MolPE..75..165P. doi:10.1016/j.ympev.2014.02.023. ISSN 1055-7903. PMC 4059600. PMID 24583291.
  30. Stanyon, Roscoe; Finstermeier, Knut; Zinner, Dietmar; Brameier, Markus; Meyer, Matthias; Kreuz, Eva; Hofreiter, Michael; Roos, Christian (16 July 2013). "A Mitogenomic Phylogeny of Living Primates". PLOS ONE. 8 (7): e69504. Bibcode:2013PLoSO...869504F. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0069504. ISSN 1932-6203. PMC 3713065. PMID 23874967.
  31. O'Leary, M. A.; et al. (8 February 2013). "The placental mammal ancestor and the post–K-Pg radiation of placentals". Science. 339 (6120): 662–667. Bibcode:2013Sci...339..662O. doi:10.1126/science.1229237. hdl:11336/7302. PMID 23393258. S2CID 206544776.
  32. Wilson Mantilla, Gregory P.; Chester, Stephen G. B.; Clemens, William A.; Moore, Jason R.; Sprain, Courtney J.; Hovatter, Brody T.; Mitchell, William S.; Mans, Wade W.; Mundil, Roland; Renne, Paul R. (2021). "Earliest Palaeocene purgatoriids and the initial radiation of stem primates". Royal Society Open Science. 8 (2): 210050. Bibcode:2021RSOS....810050W. doi:10.1098/rsos.210050. PMC 8074693. PMID 33972886.
  33. Williams, B. A.; Kay, R. F.; Kirk, E. C. (2010). "New perspectives on anthropoid origins". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 107 (11): 4797–4804. Bibcode:2010PNAS..107.4797W. doi:10.1073/pnas.0908320107. PMC 2841917. PMID 20212104.
  34. Miller, E. R.; Gunnell, G. F.; Martin, R. D. (2005). "Deep Time and the Search for Anthropoid Origins" (PDF). American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 128: 60–95. doi:10.1002/ajpa.20352. PMID 16369958.
  35. Chatterjee, Helen J; Ho, Simon Y.W.; Barnes, Ian; Groves, Colin (27 October 2009). "Estimating the phylogeny and divergence times of primates using a supermatrix approach". BMC Evolutionary Biology. 9 (1): 259. Bibcode:2009BMCEE...9..259C. doi:10.1186/1471-2148-9-259. PMC 2774700. PMID 19860891.
  36. Lee, M. (September 1999). "Molecular Clock Calibrations and Metazoan Divergence Dates". Journal of Molecular Evolution. 49 (3): 385–391. Bibcode:1999JMolE..49..385L. doi:10.1007/PL00006562. PMID 10473780. S2CID 1629316.
  37. "Scientists Push Back Primate Origins From 65 Million To 85 Million Years Ago". Science Daily. Retrieved 2008-10-24.
  38. Tavaré, S.; Marshall, C. R.; Will, O.; Soligo, C.; Martin R.D. (April 18, 2002). "Using the fossil record to estimate the age of the last common ancestor of extant primates". Nature. 416 (6882): 726–729. Bibcode:2002Natur.416..726T. doi:10.1038/416726a. PMID 11961552. S2CID 4368374.
  39. Klonisch, T.; Froehlich, C.; Tetens, F.; Fischer, B.; Hombach-Klonisch, S. (2001). "Molecular Remodeling of Members of the Relaxin Family During Primate Evolution". Molecular Biology and Evolution. 18 (3): 393–403. doi:10.1093/oxfordjournals.molbev.a003815. PMID 11230540.
  40. Horvath, J.; et al. (2008). "Development and Application of a Phylogenomic Toolkit: Resolving the Evolutionary History of Madagascar's Lemurs". Genome Research. 18 (3): 489–499. doi:10.1101/gr.7265208. PMC 2259113. PMID 18245770.
  41. Mittermeier, R.; Ganzhorn, J.; Konstant, W.; Glander, K.; Tattersall, I.; Groves, C.; Rylands, A.; Hapke, A.; Ratsimbazafy, J.; Mayor, M.; Louis, E.; Rumpler, Y.; Schwitzer, C.; Rasoloarison, R. (December 2008). "Lemur Diversity in Madagascar". International Journal of Primatology. 29 (6): 1607–1656. doi:10.1007/s10764-008-9317-y. S2CID 17614597. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2021-02-15. Retrieved 2019-09-24.
  42. Sellers, Bill (2000-10-20). "Primate Evolution" (PDF). University of Edinburgh. pp. 13–17. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2008-10-29. Retrieved 2008-10-23.
  43. Hartwig, W. (2007). "Primate Evolution". In Campbell, C.; Fuentes, A.; MacKinnon, K.; Panger, M.; Bearder, S. (eds.). Primates in Perspective. Oxford University Press. pp. 13–17. ISBN 978-0-19-517133-4.
  44. Williams, B. A.; Kay, R. F.; Christopher Kirk, E.; Ross, C. F. (2010). "Darwinius masillae is a strepsirrhine—a reply to Franzen et al. (2009)" (PDF). Journal of Human Evolution. 59 (5): 567–573, discussion 573–9. Bibcode:2010JHumE..59..567W. doi:10.1016/j.jhevol.2010.01.003. PMID 20188396. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2013-05-17. Retrieved 2015-09-04.
  45. Ciochon, R. & Fleagle, J. (1987). Primate Evolution and Human Origins. Menlo Park, California: Benjamin/Cummings. p. 72. ISBN 978-0-202-01175-2.
  46. Garbutt, N. (2007). Mammals of Madagascar, A Complete Guide. A&C Black Publishers. pp. 85–86. ISBN 978-0-300-12550-4.
  47. Mittermeier, R.A.; et al. (2006). Lemurs of Madagascar (2nd ed.). Conservation International. pp. 23–26. ISBN 1-881173-88-7.
  48. Shekelle, M. (2005). Evolutionary Biology of Tarsiers. Archived from the original on 2008-09-07. Retrieved 2008-08-22.
  49. Schmidt, T.; et al. (3 May 2005). "Rapid electrostatic evolution at the binding site for cytochrome c on cytochrome c oxidase in anthropoid primates". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 102 (18): 6379–6384. Bibcode:2005PNAS..102.6379S. doi:10.1073/pnas.0409714102. PMC 1088365. PMID 15851671.
  50. Wade, Lizzie (June 5, 2013). "Early Primate Weighed Less Than an Ounce". ScienceNow. Archived from the original on 2013-06-08. Retrieved 2013-06-07.
  51. Kay, R. F. (2012). "Evidence for an Asian origin of stem anthropoid s". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 109 (26): 10132–10133. Bibcode:2012PNAS..10910132K. doi:10.1073/pnas.1207933109. PMC 3387095. PMID 22699505.
  52. Chaimanee, Y.; Chavasseau, O.; Beard, K. C.; Kyaw, A. A.; Soe, A. N.; Sein, C.; Lazzari, V.; Marivaux, L.; Marandat, B.; Swe, M.; Rugbumrung, M.; Lwin, T.; Valentin, X.; Zin-Maung-Maung-Thein; Jaeger, J. -J. (2012). "Late Middle Eocene primate from Myanmar and the initial anthropoid colonization of Africa". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 109 (26): 10293–10297. Bibcode:2012PNAS..10910293C. doi:10.1073/pnas.1200644109. PMC 3387043. PMID 22665790.
  53. Marivaux, L.; et al. (2005-06-14). "Anthropoid primates from the Oligocene of Pakistan (Bugti Hills): Data on early anthropoid evolution and biogeography". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 102 (24): 8436–8441. Bibcode:2005PNAS..102.8436M. doi:10.1073/pnas.0503469102. PMC 1150860. PMID 15937103.
  54. Schrago, C.G. & Russo, C.A.M. (2003). "Timing the Origin of New World Monkeys" (PDF Reprint). Molecular Biology and Evolution. 20 (10): 1620–1625. doi:10.1093/molbev/msg172. PMID 12832653.
  55. Houle, A. (1999). "The origin of platyrrhines: An evaluation of the Antarctic scenario and the floating island model". American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 109 (4): 541–559. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1096-8644(199908)109:4<541::AID-AJPA9>3.0.CO;2-N. PMID 10423268.
  56. Andrews, P. & Kelley, J. (2007). "Middle Miocene Dispersals of Apes". Folia Primatologica. 78 (5–6): 328–343. doi:10.1159/000105148. PMID 17855786. S2CID 19293586.
  57. Strier, K. (2007). Primate Behavioral Ecology (3rd ed.). Allyn & Bacon. pp. 7, 64, 71, 77, 182–185, 273–280, 284, 287–298. ISBN 978-0-205-44432-8.
  58. Pough, F. W.; Janis, C. M.; Heiser, J. B. (2005) [1979]. "Primate Evolution and the Emergence of Humans". Vertebrate Life (7th ed.). Pearson. pp. 650. ISBN 0-13-127836-3.
  59. IUCN/SSC Primate Specialist Group (1 March 2021). "Primate diversity by region". International Union for the Conservation of Nature.
  60. Tenaza, R. (1984). "Songs of hybrid gibbons (Hylobates lar × H. muelleri)". American Journal of Primatology. 8 (3): 249–253. doi:10.1002/ajp.1350080307. PMID 31986810. S2CID 84957700.
  61. Bernsteil, I. S. (1966). "Naturally occurring primate hybrid". Science. 154 (3756): 1559–1560. Bibcode:1966Sci...154.1559B. doi:10.1126/science.154.3756.1559. PMID 4958933. S2CID 85898043.
  62. Sugawara, K. (January 1979). "Sociological study of a wild group of hybrid baboons between Papio anubis and P. hamadryas in the Awash Valley, Ethiopia". Primates. 20 (1): 21–56. doi:10.1007/BF02373827. S2CID 23061688.
  63. Jolly, C. J.; Woolley-Barker, Tamsin; et al. (1997). "Intergeneric Hybrid Baboons". International Journal of Primatology. 18 (4): 597–627. doi:10.1023/A:1026367307470. S2CID 27900830.
  64. Liu, Zhen; et al. (24 January 2018). "Cloning of Macaque Monkeys by Somatic Cell Nuclear Transfer". Cell. 172 (4): 881–887.e7. doi:10.1016/j.cell.2018.01.020. PMID 29395327.
  65. Normile, Dennis (24 January 2018). "These monkey twins are the first primate clones made by the method that developed Dolly". Science. doi:10.1126/science.aat1066. Retrieved 24 January 2018.
  66. Cyranoski, David (24 January 2018). "First monkeys cloned with technique that made Dolly the sheep - Chinese scientists create cloned primates that could revolutionize studies of human disease". Nature. 553 (7689): 387–388. Bibcode:2018Natur.553..387C. doi:10.1038/d41586-018-01027-z. PMID 29368720.
  67. Briggs, Helen (24 January 2018). "First monkey clones created in Chinese laboratory". BBC News. Retrieved 24 January 2018.
  68. "Scientists Successfully Clone Monkeys; Are Humans Up Next?". The New York Times. Associated Press. 24 January 2018. Retrieved 24 January 2018.
  69. Pough, F. W.; Janis, C. M.; Heiser, J. B. (2005) [1979]. "Characteristics of Primates". Vertebrate Life (7th ed.). Pearson. pp. 630. ISBN 0-13-127836-3.
  70. Aiello, L. & Dean, C. (1990). An Introduction to Human Evolutionary Anatomy. Academic Press. pp. 193. ISBN 0-12-045590-0.
  71. "Primate". Encyclopædia Britannica Online. Encyclopædia Britannica, Inc. 2008. Retrieved 2008-07-21.
  72. Myers, P. (1999). ""Primates" (On-line)". Animal Diversity Web. Retrieved 2008-06-03.
  73. Caves, Eleanor M. (May 2018). "Visual Acuity and the Evolution of Signals". Trends in Ecology & Evolution. 33 (5): 358–372. Bibcode:2018TEcoE..33..358C. doi:10.1016/j.tree.2018.03.001. PMID 29609907. Retrieved 29 July 2018.
  74. Kirk, E. Christopher; Kay, Richard F. (2004), Ross, Callum F.; Kay, Richard F. (eds.), "The Evolution of High Visual Acuity in the Anthropoidea", Anthropoid Origins: New Visions, Developments in Primatology: Progress and Prospects, Boston, MA: Springer US, pp. 539–602, doi:10.1007/978-1-4419-8873-7_20, ISBN 978-1-4419-8873-7, retrieved 2023-07-30
  75. Campbell, B. G. & Loy, J. D. (2000). Humankind Emerging (8th ed.). Allyn & Bacon. p. 85. ISBN 0-673-52364-0.
  76. White, T. & Kazlev, A. (2006-01-08). "Archonta: Primates". Palaeos. Archived from the original on 2008-05-12. Retrieved 2008-06-03.
  77. Macdonald, David (2006). "Primates". The Encyclopedia of Mammals. The Brown Reference Group plc. pp. 282–307. ISBN 0-681-45659-0.
  78. Ash, M. M.; Nelson, S. J.; Wheeler, R. C. (2003). Wheeler's Dental Anatomy, Physiology, and Occlusion. W.B. Saunders. p. 12. ISBN 978-0-7216-9382-8.
  79. Garber PA, Rehg JA (November 1999). "The ecological role of the prehensile tail in white-faced capuchins (Cebus capucinus)". American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 110 (3): 325–39. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1096-8644(199911)110:3<325::AID-AJPA5>3.0.CO;2-D. PMID 10516564.
  80. Russo GA, Young JW (November 2011). "Tail growth tracks the ontogeny of prehensile tail use in capuchin monkeys (Cebus albifrons and C. apella)". American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 146 (3): 465–73. doi:10.1002/ajpa.21617. PMID 21953012.
  81. Friderun Ankel-Simons (27 July 2010). Primate Anatomy: An Introduction. Academic Press. pp. 442, 521. ISBN 978-0-08-046911-9.
  82. Ralls, K. (1976). "Mammals in Which Females are Larger Than Males". The Quarterly Review of Biology. 51 (2): 245–76. doi:10.1086/409310. PMID 785524. S2CID 25927323.
  83. Lindstedtand & Boyce; Boyce, Mark S. (July 1985). "Seasonality, Fasting Endurance, and Body Size in Mammals". The American Naturalist. 125 (6): 873. Bibcode:1985ANat..125..873L. doi:10.1086/284385. S2CID 84308684.
  84. Frisch, J. E. (1963). "Sex-differences in the canines of the gibbon (Hylobates lar)". Primates. 4 (2): 1–10. doi:10.1007/BF01659148. S2CID 189798134.
  85. Kay, R. F. (1975). "The functional adaptations of primate molar teeth". American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 43 (2): 195–215. doi:10.1002/ajpa.1330430207. PMID 810034.
  86. Crook, J. H. (1972). "Sexual selection, dimorphism, and social organization in the primates". In Campbell, B. G. (ed.). Sexual selection and the descent of man. Aldine Transaction. pp. 246. ISBN 978-0-202-02005-1.
  87. Cheverud, J. M.; Dow, M. M.; Leutenegger, W. (November 1985). "The quantitative assessment of phylogenetic constraints in comparative analyses: Sexual dimorphism in body weight among primates". Evolution. 39 (6): 1335–1351. doi:10.2307/2408790. JSTOR 2408790. PMID 28564267.
  88. Leutenegger, W.; Cheverud, J. M. (1982). "Correlates of sexual dimorphism in primates: Ecological and size variables". International Journal of Primatology. 3 (4): 387–402. doi:10.1007/BF02693740. S2CID 38220186.
  89. Plavcan, J. M. (2001). "Sexual dimorphism in primate evolution". American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 33: 25–53. doi:10.1002/ajpa.10011. PMID 11786990. S2CID 31722173.
  90. O'Higgins, P.; Collard, M. (2002). "Sexual dimorphism and facial growth in papionine monkeys". Journal of Zoology. 257 (2): 255–72. doi:10.1017/S0952836902000857.
  91. Sussman, R. W. (1999). Primate Ecology and Social Structure Volume 1: Lorises, Lemurs and Tarsiers. Needham Heights, MA: Pearson Custom Publishing & Prentice Hall. pp. 78, 89–90, 108, 121–123, 233. ISBN 0-536-02256-9.
  92. Sussman, R. W. (2003). Primate Ecology and Social Structure, Volume 2: New World Monkeys (Revised First ed.). Needham Heights, MA: Pearson Custom Publishing & Prentice Hall. pp. 77–80, 132–133, 141–143. ISBN 0-536-74364-9.
  93. Glazier, S. D.; Flowerday, C. A. (2003). Selected Readings in the Anthropology of Religion: Theoretical and Methodological Essays. Greenwood Publishing Group. pp. 53. ISBN 9780313300905.
  94. Arrese, C. A.; Oddy, Alison Y.; et al. (2005). "Cone topography and spectral sensitivity in two potentially trichromatic marsupials, the quokka (Setonix brachyurus) and quenda (Isoodon obesulus)". Proceedings of the Royal Society B. 272 (1565): 791–6. doi:10.1098/rspb.2004.3009. PMC 1599861. PMID 15888411.
  95. Bowmaker, J. K.; Astell, S.; Hunt, D. M.; Mollon, J. D. (1991). "Photosensitive and photostable pigments in the retinae of Old World monkeys" (PDF). The Journal of Experimental Biology. 156 (1): 1–19. Bibcode:1991JExpB.156....1B. doi:10.1242/jeb.156.1.1. ISSN 0022-0949. PMID 2051127. Retrieved 2008-06-16.
  96. Surridge, A. K. & D. Osorio (2003). "Evolution and selection of trichromatic vision in primates". Trends in Ecology and Evolution. 18 (4): 198–205. doi:10.1016/S0169-5347(03)00012-0.
  97. Lucas, P. W.; Dominy, N. J.; Riba-Hernandez, P.; Stoner, K. E.; Yamashita, N.; Loría-Calderón, E.; Petersen-Pereira, W.; Rojas-Durán, Y.; Salas-Pena, R.; Solis-Madrigal, S.; Osorio, D.; Darvell, B. W. (2003). "Evolution and function of routine trichromatic vision in primates". Evolution. 57 (11): 2636–43. doi:10.1554/03-168. PMID 14686538. S2CID 739130.
  98. Wrangham, R. W. (1982). "Mutualism, kinship and social evolution". Current Problems in Sociobiology. Cambridge University Press. pp. 269–89. ISBN 0-521-24203-7.
  99. Goldberg, T. L.; Wrangham, R. W. (September 1997). "Genetic correlates of social behavior in wild chimpanzees: evidence from mitochondrial DNA". Animal Behaviour. 54 (3): 559–70. doi:10.1006/anbe.1996.0450. PMID 9299041. S2CID 18223362.
  100. Bowdler, Neil (21 December 2010). "Neanderthal family found cannibalised in cave in Spain". BBC News.
  101. Bowdler, Neil (2 June 2011). "Ancient cave women 'left childhood homes'". BBC News. Retrieved 2 June 2011.
  102. Copeland, Sandi R.; et al. (1 June 2011). "Strontium isotope evidence for landscape use by early hominins". Nature. 474 (7349): 76–78. doi:10.1038/nature10149. PMID 21637256. S2CID 205225222.
  103. Fiore, A. D. & Campbell, C. J. (2007). "The Atelines". In Campbell, C. J.; Fuentes, A.; MacKinnon, K. C.; Panger, M. & Bearder, S. K. (eds.). Primates in Perspective. Oxford University Press. p. 175. ISBN 978-0-19-517133-4.
  104. Bartlett, T. Q. (2007). "The Hylobatidae". In Campbell, C. J.; Fuentes, A.; MacKinnon, K. C.; Panger, M.; Bearder, S. K. (eds.). Primates in Perspective. Oxford University Press. p. 283. ISBN 978-0-19-517133-4.
  105. Dixon, Alan F (2012). Primate Sexuality. Oxford University Press. pp. 32–61. ISBN 9780199544646.
  106. Watts D. P. (1996). "Comparative socio-ecology of gorillas". In McGrew W. C.; Marchant L. F.; Nishida, T. (eds.). Great Ape Societies. Cambridge (England: Cambridge Univ Press. pp. 16–28. ISBN 978-0521555364.
  107. Charpentier MJ, Widdig A, Alberts SC (December 2007). "Inbreeding depression in non-human primates: a historical review of methods used and empirical data". American Journal of Primatology. 69 (12): 1370–86. doi:10.1002/ajp.20445. PMID 17486606. S2CID 46626761.
  108. Ralls K, Ballou J (1982). "Effect of inbreeding on infant mortality in captive primates" (PDF). International Journal of Primatology. 3 (4): 491–505. doi:10.1007/BF02693747. S2CID 10954608.[permanent dead link]
  109. Constable, J. L.; Ashley, M. V.; Goodall, J.; Pusey, A. E. (May 2001). "Noninvasive paternity assignment in Gombe chimpanzees". Molecular Ecology. 10 (5): 1279–300. Bibcode:2001MolEc..10.1279C. doi:10.1046/j.1365-294X.2001.01262.x. PMID 11380884. S2CID 46604532.
  110. Rowe, N. (1996). The Pictorial Guide to the Living Primates. Pogonias Press. pp. 4, 139, 143, 15 185, 223. ISBN 0-9648825-0-7.
  111. Couzin, Iain D.; Laidre, Mark E. (August 2009). "Fission–fusion populations". Current Biology. 19 (15): R633 – R635. Bibcode:2009CBio...19.R633C. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2009.05.034. ISSN 0960-9822. PMID 19674541. S2CID 13549970.
  112. Pough, F. W.; Janis, C. M.; Heiser, J. B. (2005) [1979]. "Primate Societies". Vertebrate Life (7th ed.). Pearson. pp. 621–623. ISBN 0-13-127836-3.
  113. Smuts, B.B., Cheney, D.L. Seyfarth, R.M., Wrangham, R.W., & Struhsaker, T.T. (Eds.) (1987). Primate Societies. Chicago: University of Chicago Press for articles on the structure and function of various primate societies.
  114. Zimmer, Carl (16 November 2023). "Scientists Find First Evidence That Groups of Apes Cooperate - Some bonobos are challenging the notion that humans are the only primates capable of group-to-group alliances". The New York Times. Archived from the original on 16 November 2023. Retrieved 17 November 2023.
  115. Samuni, Liran; et al. (16 November 2023). "Cooperation across social borders in bonobos". Science. 382 (6672): 805–809. Bibcode:2023Sci...382..805S. doi:10.1126/science.adg0844. PMID 37972165. Archived from the original on 17 November 2023. Retrieved 17 November 2023.
  116. Shultz, S. & Thomsett, S. (2007). "Interactions between African Crowned Eagles and Their Prey Community". In McGraw, W.; Zuberbuhler, K. & Noe, R. (eds.). Monkeys of Tai Forest, An African Primate Community. Cambridge University Press. p. 181. ISBN 978-0-521-81633-5.
  117. Bshary, R. (2007). "Interactions between Red Colobus Monkeys and Chimpanzees". In McGraw, W.; Zuberbuhler, K.; Noe, R. (eds.). Monkeys of Tai Forest, An African Primate Community. Cambridge University Press. pp. 155–170. ISBN 978-0-521-81633-5.
  118. Stanford, C. (1998). Chimpanzee and Red Colobus : the ecology of predator and prey. Harvard University Press. pp. 130–138, 233. ISBN 0-674-00722-0.
  119. Boinski, S. (2000). "Social Manipulation Within and Between Troops Mediates Primate Group Movement". In Boinski, S.; Garber, P. (eds.). On the Move : how and why animals travel in groups. University of Chicago Press. pp. 447–448. ISBN 0-226-06340-2.
  120. Dupanloup, Isabelle; Pereira, Luisa; Bertorelle, Giorgio; Calafell, Francesc; Prata, Maria João; Amorim, Antonio; Barbujani, Guido (1 July 2003). "A Recent Shift from Polygyny to Monogamy in Humans Is Suggested by the Analysis of Worldwide Y-Chromosome Diversity". Journal of Molecular Evolution. 57 (1): 85–97. Bibcode:2003JMolE..57...85D. doi:10.1007/s00239-003-2458-x. ISSN 0022-2844. PMID 12962309. Retrieved 13 July 2024 – via Springer Link.
  121. Dixon, Alan F (2012). Primate Sexuality. Oxford University Press. pp. 130–131, 135, 149–156, 200–202. ISBN 9780199544646.
  122. de Waal FB (March 1995). "Bonobo sex and society" (PDF). Scientific American. 272 (3): 82–8. Bibcode:1995SciAm.272c..82W. doi:10.1038/scientificamerican0395-82. PMID 7871411. Archived from the original (PDF) on 27 January 2012. Retrieved 21 December 2011.
  123. Opie, Christopher; Atkinson, Quentin D.; Dunbarc, Robin I. M.; Shultz, Susanne (2013). "Male infanticide leads to social monogamy in primates". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 110 (33): 13328–13332. Bibcode:2013PNAS..11013328O. doi:10.1073/pnas.1307903110. PMC 3746880. PMID 23898180.
  124. De Ruiter, Jan R.; Van Hooff, Jan A. R. A. M. & Scheffrahn, Wolfgang (1994). "Social and genetic aspects of paternity in wild long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis)". Behaviour. 129 (3–4): 203–24. doi:10.1163/156853994x00613. JSTOR 4535195.
  125. Kappeler, Peter M. (1998). "Nests, Tree Holes, and the Evolution of Primate Life Histories". American Journal of Primatology. 46 (1): 7–33. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1098-2345(1998)46:1<7::AID-AJP3>3.0.CO;2-#. PMID 9730211. S2CID 196589387.
  126. Ross, Caroline (1991). "Park or ride? Evolution of infant carrying in primates". International Journal of Primatology. 22 (5). Kluwer Academic Publishing: 749–771. doi:10.1023/A:1012065332758. S2CID 25301078.
  127. Mintz, Zoe (14 January 2014). "Humans And Primates Burn 50 Percent Fewer Calories Each Day Than Other Mammals". www.ibtimes.com. IBT Media Inc. Retrieved 2014-01-14.
  128. Walker ML, Herndon JG; Herndon (2008). "Menopause in nonhuman primates?". Biology of Reproduction. 79 (3): 398–406. doi:10.1095/biolreprod.108.068536. PMC 2553520. PMID 18495681.
  129. Milton, K. (1993). "Diet and Primate Evolution" (PDF). Scientific American. Vol. 269, no. 2. pp. 86–93. Bibcode:1993SciAm.269b..86M. doi:10.1038/scientificamerican0893-86. PMID 8351513.
  130. Pollock, J. I.; Mullin, R. J. (1986). "Vitamin C biosynthesis in prosimians: Evidence for the anthropoid affinity of Tarsius". American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 73 (1): 65–70. doi:10.1002/ajpa.1330730106. PMID 3113259. Archived from the original on 2012-06-28. Retrieved 2010-03-16.
  131. Milliken, G. W.; Ward, J. P.; Erickson, C. J. (1991). "Independent digit control in foraging by the aye-aye (Daubentonia madagascariensis)". Folia Primatologica. 56 (4): 219–224. doi:10.1159/000156551. PMID 1937286.
  132. Hiller, C. (2000). "Theropithecus gelada". Animal Diversity Web. Retrieved 2008-08-08.
  133. Wright, P.; Simmons, E.; Gursky, S. (2003). "Introduction". In Wright, P.; Simmons, E.; Gursky, S. (eds.). Tarsiers Past, Present and Future. Rutgers University Press. p. 1. ISBN 0-8135-3236-1.
  134. Goodall, Jane (1986). The Chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of Behavior. Belknap Press of Harvard University Press. ISBN 0-674-11649-6.
  135. Guernsey, Paul. "WHAT DO CHIMPS EAT?". All About Wildlife. Archived from the original on 2019-11-18. Retrieved 2013-04-22.
  136. Ihobe H (1992). "Observations on the meat-eating behavior of wild bonobos (Pan paniscus) at Wamba, Republic of Zaire". Primates. 33 (2): 247–250. doi:10.1007/BF02382754. S2CID 10063791.
  137. Rafert, J.; Vineberg, E.O. (1997). "Bonobo Nutrition – relation of captive diet to wild diet" (PDF). Bonobo Husbandry Manual. American Association of Zoos and Aquariums. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2012-04-25.
  138. Surbeck, M; Fowler, A; Deimel, C; Hohmann, G (2008). "Evidence for the consumption of arboreal, diurnal primates by bonobos (Pan paniscus)". American Journal of Primatology. 71 (2): 171–4. doi:10.1002/ajp.20634. PMID 19058132. S2CID 32622605.
  139. Surbeck M, Hohmann G; Hohmann (14 October 2008). "Primate hunting by bonobos at LuiKotale, Salonga National Park". Current Biology. 18 (19): R906–7. Bibcode:2008CBio...18.R906S. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2008.08.040. PMID 18957233. S2CID 6708310.
  140. Cordain L, Eaton SB, Sebastian A, et al. (February 2005). "Origins and evolution of the Western diet: health implications for the 21st century". Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 81 (2): 341–54. doi:10.1093/ajcn.81.2.341. PMID 15699220.
  141. Ulijaszek SJ (November 2002). "Human eating behaviour in an evolutionary ecological context". Proc Nutr Soc. 61 (4): 517–26. doi:10.1079/PNS2002180. PMID 12691181.
  142. Earliest agriculture in the Americas Archived 3 June 2010 at the Wayback Machine Earliest cultivation of barley Archived 16 February 2007 at the Wayback Machine Earliest cultivation of figs Archived 2 June 2006 at the Wayback Machine, retrieved 19 February 2007
  143. Krebs JR (September 2009). "The gourmet ape: evolution and human food preferences". Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 90 (3): 707S – 11S. doi:10.3945/ajcn.2009.27462B. PMID 19656837.
  144. Holden C, Mace R (October 1997). "Phylogenetic analysis of the evolution of lactose digestion in adults". Hum. Biol. 69 (5): 605–28. PMID 9299882.
  145. Fichtel, Claudia (2012). "Predation". In Mitani, John C.; Call, Josep; Kappeler, Peter M.; Palombit, Ryne A.; Silk, Joan B. (eds.). The Evolution of Primate Societies. University of Chicago Press. pp. 169–84. ISBN 978-0-226-53172-4.
  146. Liman, E. R.; Innan, H. (2003). "Relaxed selective pressure on an essential component of pheromone transduction in primate evolution" (PDF). Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 100 (6): 3328–3332. Bibcode:2003PNAS..100.3328L. doi:10.1073/pnas.0636123100. PMC 152292. PMID 12631698. Retrieved 2008-07-23.
  147. Egnor, R.; Miller, C.; Hauser, M.D. (2004). "Nonhuman Primate Communication" (PDF). Encyclopedia of Language and Linguistics (2nd ed.). Elsevier. ISBN 0-08-044299-4. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2008-09-10.
  148. Pollick, A. S.; de Waal, F. B. M. (2007). "Ape gestures and language evolution". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 104 (19): 8184–8189. Bibcode:2007PNAS..104.8184P. doi:10.1073/pnas.0702624104. PMC 1876592. PMID 17470779.
  149. Burrows, A. M. (2008). "The facial expression musculature in primates and its evolutionary significance". BioEssays. 30 (3): 212–225. doi:10.1002/bies.20719. PMID 18293360. S2CID 205478149.
  150. Wright, E.; Grawunder, S; Ndayishimiye, E; Galbany, J; McFarlin, S. C.; Stoinski, T. S.; Robbins, M. M. (2021). "Chest beats as an honest signal of body size in male mountain gorillas (Gorilla beringei beringei)". Scientific Reports. 11 (1): 6879. Bibcode:2021NatSR..11.6879W. doi:10.1038/s41598-021-86261-8. PMC 8032651. PMID 33833252.
  151. Geissmann, Thomas & Mutschler, Thomas (2006). "Diurnal Distribution of Loud Calls in Sympatric Wild Indris (Indri indri) and Ruffed Lemurs (Varecia variegata): Implications for Call Functions" (PDF). Primates; Journal of Primatology. 47 (4): 393–6. doi:10.1007/s10329-006-0189-5. PMID 16736264. S2CID 1586657.
  152. Ramsier, M.A.; Cunningham, A.J.; Moritz, G.L.; Finneran, J.J.; Williams, C.V.; Ong, P.S.; Gursky-Doyen, S.L.; Dominy, N.J. (2012). "Primate communication in the pure ultrasound". Biology Letters. 8 (4): 508–511. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2011.1149. PMC 3391437. PMID 22319094.
  153. da Cunha, R. G. T.; Byrne, R. (2006). "Roars of Black Howler Monkeys (Alouatta caraya): Evidence for a Function in Inter-Group Spacing". Behaviour. 143 (10): 1169–1199. doi:10.1163/156853906778691568. JSTOR 4536401.
  154. "Black howler monkey". Smithsonian's National Zoo & Conservation Biology Institute. 25 April 2016. Retrieved 2016-07-10.
  155. Haimoff, E. H. (1983). "Brief report: Occurrence of anti-resonance in the song of the siamang (Hylobates syndactylus)". American Journal of Primatology. 5 (3): 249–256. doi:10.1002/ajp.1350050309. PMID 31986856. S2CID 85262432.
  156. Seyfarth, R. M.; Cheney, D. L.; Marler, Peter (1980). "Vervet Monkey Alarm Calls: Semantic communication in a Free-Ranging Primate". Animal Behaviour. 28 (4): 1070–1094. doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(80)80097-2. S2CID 53165940.
  157. Leroux, M.; Schel, A.M.; Wilke, C.; Chandia, B.; Zuberbuhler, K.; Slocombe, K.E.; Townsend, S. (2023). "Call combinations and compositional processing in wild chimpanzees". Nature Communications. 14 (1): 2225. Bibcode:2023NatCo..14.2225L. doi:10.1038/s41467-023-37816-y. PMC 10160036. PMID 37142584.
  158. Coye, C.; Ouattara, K.; Zuberbuhler, K.; Lemasson, A. (2015). "Suffixation influences receivers' behaviour in non-human primates". Proceedings of the Royal Society B. 282 (1807): 20150265. doi:10.1098/rspb.2015.0265. PMC 4424650. PMID 25925101.
  159. Coye, C.; Zuberbuhler, K.; Lemasson, A. (2016). "Morphologically structured vocalizations in female Diana monkeys". Animal Behaviour. 115: 97–105. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2016.03.010. hdl:10023/10629.
  160. Leroux, M.; Townsend, S. (2020). "Call Combinations in Great Apes and the Evolution of Syntax". Animal Behavior and Cognition. 7 (2): 131–139. doi:10.26451/abc.07.02.07.2020.
  161. Lameira, A. R.; et al. (2021). "Orangutan information broadcast via consonant-like and vowel-like calls breaches mathematical models of linguistic evolution". Biology Letters. 17 (9). doi:10.1098/rsbl.2021.0302. PMC 8478518. PMID 34582737.
  162. Arcadi, AC. (Aug 2000). "Vocal responsiveness in male wild chimpanzees: implications for the evolution of language". J Hum Evol. 39 (2): 205–23. Bibcode:2000JHumE..39..205A. doi:10.1006/jhev.2000.0415. PMID 10968929. S2CID 7403772.
  163. Boesch, C.; Boesch, H. (1990). "Tool Use and Tool Making in Wild Chimpanzees". Folia Primatologica. 54 (1–2): 86–99. doi:10.1159/000156428. PMID 2157651.
  164. Westergaard, G. C.; Lundquist, A. L.; et al. (1998). "Why some capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) use probing tools (and others do not)". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 112 (2): 207–211. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.112.2.207. PMID 9642788.
  165. de Waal, F. B. M.; Davis, J. M. (2003). "Capuchin cognitive ecology: cooperation based on projected returns". Neuropsychologia. 41 (2): 221–228. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00152-5. PMID 12459220. S2CID 8190458.
  166. Paar, L. A.; Winslow, J. T.; Hopkins, W. D.; de Waal, F. B. M. (2000). "Recognizing facial cues: Individual discrimination by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta)". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 114 (1): 47–60. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.114.1.47. PMC 2018744. PMID 10739311.
  167. Byrne, Richard; Corp, Nadia (2004). "Neocortex size predicts deception rate in primates". Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences. 271 (1549): 1693–1699. doi:10.1098/rspb.2004.2780. PMC 1691785. PMID 15306289.
  168. Paar, L. A.; de Waal, F. B. M. (1999). "Visual kin recognition in chimpanzees". Nature. 399 (6737): 647–648. Bibcode:1999Natur.399..647P. doi:10.1038/21345. PMID 10385114. S2CID 4424086.
  169. Fujita, K.; Watanabe, K.; Widarto, T. H.; Suryobroto, B. (1997). "Discrimination of macaques by macaques: The case of sulawesi species". Primates. 38 (3): 233–245. doi:10.1007/BF02381612. S2CID 21042762.
  170. Call, J. (2001). "Object permanence in orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus), chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), and children (Homo sapiens)". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 115 (2): 159–171. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.115.2.159. PMID 11459163.
  171. Itakura, S.; Tanaka, M. (June 1998). "Use of experimenter-given cues during object-choice tasks by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), an orangutan (Pongo pygmaeus), and human infants (Homo sapiens)". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 112 (2): 119–126. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.112.2.119. PMID 9642782.
  172. Gouteux, S.; Thinus-Blanc, C.; Vauclair, J. (2001). "Rhesus monkeys use geometric and nongeometric information during a reorientation task" (PDF). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 130 (3): 505–519. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.130.3.505. PMID 11561924.
  173. Tomasello, M. & Call, J. (1997). Primate Cognition. Oxford University Press US. ISBN 978-0-19-510624-4.
  174. Deaner, R. O.; van Schaik, C. P.; Johnson, V. E. (2006). "Do some taxa have better domain-general cognition than others? A metaanalysis of nonhuman primate studies". Evolutionary Psychology. 4: 149–196. doi:10.1177/147470490600400114. S2CID 16702785.
  175. Reader, S. M.; Hager, Y.; Laland, K. N. (2011). "The evolution of primate general and cultural intelligence" (PDF). Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B. 366 (1567): 1017–1027. doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0342. PMC 3049098. PMID 21357224. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2011-10-03. Retrieved 2011-07-04.
  176. "Toolmaking". The Jane Goodall Institute. Retrieved 2013-08-01.
  177. "Bonobos". ApeTag. 2010. Archived from the original on 2013-11-02. Retrieved 2013-08-03.
  178. Gruber, T.; Clay, Z.; Zuberbühler, K. (2010). "A comparison of bonobo and chimpanzee tool use: evidence for a female bias in the Pan lineage" (PDF). Animal Behaviour. 80 (6): 1023–1033. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.09.005. S2CID 14923158.
  179. Bower, B. (18 April 2011). "Orangutans use simple tools to catch fish". Wired. Retrieved 2013-08-05.
  180. Breuer, T.; Ndoundou-Hockemba, M.; Fishlock, V. (2005). "First observation of tool use in wild gorillas". PLOS Biology. 3 (11): e380. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0030380. PMC 1236726. PMID 16187795.
  181. Boinski, S. (1988). "Use of a club by a wild white-faced capuchin (Cebus capucinus) to attack a venomous snake (Bothrops asper)". American Journal of Primatology. 14 (2): 177–179. doi:10.1002/ajp.1350140208. PMID 31973450. S2CID 84653622.
  182. Fragaszy, D.; Izar, P.; Visalberghi, E.; Ottoni, E.B.; de Oliveira, M.G. (2004). "Wild capuchin monkeys (Cebus libidinosus) use anvils and stone pounding tools". American Journal of Primatology. 64 (4): 359–366. doi:10.1002/ajp.20085. PMID 15580579. S2CID 16222308.
  183. Gumert, M.D.; Kluck, M.; Malaivijitnond, S. (2009). "The physical characteristics and usage patterns of stone axe and pounding hammers used by long-tailed macaques in the Andaman Sea region of Thailand". American Journal of Primatology. 71 (7): 594–608. doi:10.1002/ajp.20694. PMID 19405083. S2CID 22384150.
  184. Hamilton, W.J.; Buskirk, R.E.; Buskirk, W.H. (1975). "Defensive stoning by baboons". Nature. 256 (5517): 488–489. Bibcode:1975Natur.256..488H. doi:10.1038/256488a0. S2CID 4149862.
  185. Fichtel, C.; Kappeler, P. M. (2010). "Chapter 19: Human universals and primate symplesiomorphies: Establishing the lemur baseline". In Kappeler, P. M.; Silk, J. B. (eds.). Mind the Gap: Tracing the Origins of Human Universals. Springer. ISBN 978-3-642-02724-6.
  186. Sugiyama, Y. (1995). "Drinking tools of wild chimpanzees at Bossou". American Journal of Primatology. 37 (1): 263–269. doi:10.1002/ajp.1350370308. PMID 31936951. S2CID 86473603.
  187. "Sumatran orangutans". OrangutanIslands.com. Archived from the original on 2013-11-26. Retrieved 2013-08-02.
  188. van Schaik, C.; Fox, E.; Sitompul, A. (1996). "Manufacture and use of tools in wild Sumatran orangutans". Naturwissenschaften. 83 (4): 186–188. Bibcode:1996NW.....83..186V. doi:10.1007/BF01143062. PMID 8643126. S2CID 27180148.
  189. Gill, Victoria (22 July 2011). "Mandrill monkey makes 'pedicuring' tool". BBC. Retrieved 2013-08-11.
  190. Vancatova, M. (2008). "Gorillas and Tools – Part I". Retrieved 2013-08-04.

Primates is an order of mammals which is further divided into the strepsirrhines which include lemurs galagos and lorisids and the haplorhines which include tarsiers and simians monkeys and apes Primates arose 74 63 million years ago first from small terrestrial mammals which adapted for life in tropical forests many primate characteristics represent adaptations to the challenging environment among tree tops including large brain sizes binocular vision color vision vocalizations shoulder girdles allowing a large degree of movement in the upper limbs and opposable thumbs in most but not all that enable better grasping and dexterity Primates range in size from Madame Berthe s mouse lemur which weighs 30 g 1 oz to the eastern gorilla weighing over 200 kg 440 lb There are 376 524 species of living primates depending on which classification is used New primate species continue to be discovered over 25 species were described in the 2000s 36 in the 2010s and six in the 2020s Primates Temporal range 65 9 0 Ma PreꞒ Ꞓ O S D C P T J K Pg N Early Paleocene to PresentRing tailed lemurRed slender lorisNorthern greater galagoPhilippine tarsierBlack spider monkeyHamadryas baboonLar gibbonHumansScientific classificationDomain EukaryotaKingdom AnimaliaPhylum ChordataClass MammaliaMirorder PrimatomorphaOrder Primates Linnaeus 1758SubordersStrepsirrhini Haplorhini AltiatlasiusRange and density of non human primates SynonymsPlesiadapiformes cladistically including crown primates Primates have large brains relative to body size compared to other mammals as well as an increased reliance on visual acuity at the expense of the sense of smell which is the dominant sensory system in most mammals These features are more developed in monkeys and apes and noticeably less so in lorises and lemurs Some primates including gorillas humans and baboons are primarily ground dwelling rather than arboreal but all species have adaptations for climbing trees Arboreal locomotion techniques used include leaping from tree to tree and swinging between branches of trees brachiation terrestrial locomotion techniques include walking on two hindlimbs bipedalism and modified walking on four limbs quadrupedalism via knuckle walking Primates are among the most social of all animals forming pairs or family groups uni male harems and multi male multi female groups Non human primates have at least four types of social systems many defined by the amount of movement by adolescent females between groups Primates have slower rates of development than other similarly sized mammals reach maturity later and have longer lifespans Primates are also the most cognitively advanced animals with humans genus Homo capable of creating complex languages and sophisticated civilizations and non human primates are recorded to use tools They may communicate using facial and hand gestures smells and vocalizations Close interactions between humans and non human primates NHPs can create opportunities for the transmission of zoonotic diseases especially virus diseases including herpes measles ebola rabies and hepatitis Thousands of non human primates are used in research around the world because of their psychological and physiological similarity to humans About 60 of primate species are threatened with extinction Common threats include deforestation forest fragmentation monkey drives and primate hunting for use in medicines as pets and for food Large scale tropical forest clearing for agriculture most threatens primates EtymologyThe English name primates is derived from Old French or French primat from a noun use of Latin primat from primus prime first rank The name was given by Carl Linnaeus because he thought this the highest order of animals The relationships among the different groups of primates were not clearly understood until relatively recently so the commonly used terms are somewhat confused For example ape has been used either as an alternative for monkey or for any tailless relatively human like primate Sir Wilfrid Le Gros Clark was one of the primatologists who developed the idea of trends in primate evolution and the methodology of arranging the living members of an order into an ascending series leading to humans Commonly used names for groups of primates such as prosimians monkeys lesser apes and great apes reflect this methodology According to our current understanding of the evolutionary history of the primates several of these groups are paraphyletic or rather they do not include all the descendants of a common ancestor In contrast with Clark s methodology modern classifications typically identify or name only those groupings that are monophyletic that is such a named group includes all the descendants of the group s common ancestor All groups with scientific names are clades or monophyletic groups and the sequence of scientific classification reflects the evolutionary history of the related lineages Groups that are traditionally named are shown on the right they form an ascending series per Clark see above and several groups are paraphyletic Prosimians contain two monophyletic groups the suborder Strepsirrhini or lemurs lorises and allies as well as the tarsiers of the suborder Haplorhini it is a paraphyletic grouping because it excludes the Simiiformes which also are descendants of the common ancestor Primates Monkeys comprise two monophyletic groups New World monkeys and Old World monkeys but is paraphyletic because it excludes hominoids superfamily Hominoidea also descendants of the common ancestor Simiiformes Apes as a whole and the great apes are paraphyletic if the terms are used such that they exclude humans Thus the members of the two sets of groups and hence names do not match which causes problems in relating scientific names to common usually traditional names Consider the superfamily Hominoidea In terms of the common names on the right this group consists of apes and humans and there is no single common name for all the members of the group One remedy is to create a new common name in this case hominoids Another possibility is to expand the use of one of the traditional names For example in his 2005 book the vertebrate palaeontologist Benton wrote The apes Hominoidea today include the gibbons and orangutan the gorilla and chimpanzee and humans thereby Benton was using apes to mean hominoids In that case the group heretofore called apes must now be identified as the non human apes As of 2021 update there is no consensus as to whether to accept traditional that is common but paraphyletic names or to use monophyletic names only or to use new common names or adaptations of old ones Both competing approaches can be found in biological sources often in the same work and sometimes by the same author Thus Benton defines apes to include humans then he repeatedly uses ape like to mean like an ape rather than a human and when discussing the reaction of others to a new fossil he writes of claims that Orrorin was an ape rather than a human Classification of living primatesOrder Primates was established by Carl Linnaeus in 1758 in the tenth edition of his book Systema Naturae for the genera Homo humans Simia other apes and monkeys Lemur prosimians and Vespertilio bats In the first edition of the same book 1735 he had used the name Anthropomorpha for Homo Simia and Bradypus sloths In 1839 Henri Marie Ducrotay de Blainville following Linnaeus and aping his nomenclature established the orders Secundates including the suborders Chiroptera Insectivora and Carnivora Tertiates or Glires and Quaternates including Gravigrada Pachydermata and Ruminantia but these new taxa were not accepted Before Anderson and Jones introduced the classification of Strepsirrhini and Haplorhini in 1984 followed by McKenna and Bell s 1997 work Classification of Mammals Above the species level Primates was divided into two superfamilies Prosimii and Anthropoidea Prosimii included all of the prosimians Strepsirrhini plus the tarsiers Anthropoidea contained all of the simians The cladogram below shows one possible classification sequence of the living primates groups that use common traditional names are shown on the right Primatomorpha DermopteraPrimates Strepsirrhini Lemuriformes lemurs superfamily Lemuroidea lorises and allies superfamily Lorisoidea Haplorhini Tarsiiformes tarsiers superfamily Tarsioidea Simiiformes New World monkeys parvorder Platyrrhini Catarrhini Old World monkeys superfamily Cercopithecoidea Hominoidea gibbons family Hylobatidae Hominidae orangutans subfamily Ponginae Homininae gorillas tribe Gorillini Hominini humans g Homo chimpanzees bonobos g Pan prosimians monkeys lesser apes great apesPhylogeny and geneticsEuarchontoglires Glires Rodentia rodents Lagomorpha rabbits hares pikas Euarchonta Scandentia treeshrews Primatomorpha Dermoptera colugos Primates Plesiadapiformescrown primates Order Primates is part of the clade Euarchontoglires which is nested within the clade Eutheria of Class Mammalia Recent molecular genetic research on primates colugos and treeshrews has shown that the two species of colugos are more closely related to primates than to treeshrews even though treeshrews were at one time considered primates These three orders make up the clade Euarchonta The combination of this clade with the clade Glires composed of Rodentia and Lagomorpha forms the clade Euarchontoglires Variously both Euarchonta and Euarchontoglires are ranked as superorders Some scientists consider Dermoptera to be a suborder of Primates and use the suborder Euprimates for the true primates Evolutionary history The primate lineage is thought to go back at least near the Cretaceous Paleogene boundary or around 74 63 mya The earliest possible primate proto primate may be Purgatorius which dates back to Early Paleocene of North America 66mya The oldest known primates from the fossil record date to the Late Paleocene of Africa c 57 mya Altiatlasius or the Paleocene Eocene transition in the northern continents c 55 mya Cantius Donrussellia Altanius Plesiadapis and Teilhardina Other studies including molecular clock studies have estimated the origin of the primate branch to have been in the mid Cretaceous period around 85 mya By modern cladistic reckoning the order Primates is monophyletic The suborder Strepsirrhini the wet nosed primates is generally thought to have split off from the primitive primate line about 63 mya although earlier dates are also supported The seven strepsirrhine families are the five related lemur families and the two remaining families that include the lorisids and the galagos Older classification schemes wrap Lepilemuridae into Lemuridae and Galagidae into Lorisidae yielding a four one family distribution instead of five two as presented here During the Eocene most of the northern continents were dominated by two groups the adapiforms and the omomyids The former are considered members of Strepsirrhini but did not have a toothcomb like modern lemurs recent analysis has demonstrated that Darwinius masillae fits into this grouping The latter was closely related to tarsiers monkeys and apes How these two groups relate to extant primates is unclear Omomyids perished about 30 mya while adapiforms survived until about 10 mya According to genetic studies the lemurs of Madagascar diverged from the lorisoids approximately 75 mya These studies as well as chromosomal and molecular evidence also show that lemurs are more closely related to each other than to other strepsirrhine primates However Madagascar split from Africa 160 mya and from India 90 mya To account for these facts a founding lemur population of a few individuals is thought to have reached Madagascar from Africa via a single rafting event between 50 and 80 mya Other colonization options have been suggested such as multiple colonizations from Africa and India but none are supported by the genetic and molecular evidence Common brown lemur a strepsirrhine primate Until recently the aye aye has been difficult to place within Strepsirrhini Theories had been proposed that its family Daubentoniidae was either a lemuriform primate meaning its ancestors split from the lemur line more recently than lemurs and lorises split or a sister group to all the other strepsirrhines In 2008 the aye aye family was confirmed to be most closely related to the other Malagasy lemurs likely having descended from the same ancestral population that colonized the island Suborder Haplorhini the simple nosed or dry nosed primates is composed of two sister clades Prosimian tarsiers in the family Tarsiidae monotypic in its own infraorder Tarsiiformes represent the most basal division originating about 58 mya The earliest known haplorhine skeleton that of 55 MA old tarsier like Archicebus was found in central China supporting an already suspected Asian origin for the group The infraorder Simiiformes simian primates consisting of monkeys and apes emerged about 40 mya possibly also in Asia if so they dispersed across the Tethys Sea from Asia to Africa soon afterwards There are two simian clades both parvorders Catarrhini which developed in Africa consisting of Old World monkeys humans and the other apes and Platyrrhini which developed in South America consisting of New World monkeys A third clade which included the eosimiids developed in Asia but became extinct millions of years ago As in the case of lemurs the origin of New World monkeys is unclear Molecular studies of concatenated nuclear sequences have yielded a widely varying estimated date of divergence between platyrrhines and catarrhines ranging from 33 to 70 mya while studies based on mitochondrial sequences produce a narrower range of 35 to 43 mya The anthropoid primates possibly traversed the Atlantic Ocean from Africa to South America during the Eocene by island hopping facilitated by Atlantic Ocean ridges and a lowered sea level Alternatively a single rafting event may explain this transoceanic colonization Due to continental drift the Atlantic Ocean was not nearly as wide at the time as it is today Research suggests that a small 1 kg 2 2 lb primate could have survived 13 days on a raft of vegetation Given estimated current and wind speeds this would have provided enough time to make the voyage between the continents Emperor tamarin a New World monkey Apes and monkeys spread from Africa into Europe and Asia starting in the Miocene Soon after the lorises and tarsiers made the same journey The first hominin fossils were discovered in northern Africa and date back 5 8 mya Old World monkeys disappeared from Europe about 1 8 mya Molecular and fossil studies generally show that modern humans originated in Africa 100 000 200 000 years ago Although primates are well studied in comparison to other animal groups several new species have been discovered recently and genetic tests have revealed previously unrecognised species in known populations Primate Taxonomy listed about 350 species of primates in 2001 the author Colin Groves increased that number to 376 for his contribution to the third edition of Mammal Species of the World MSW3 However publications since the taxonomy in MSW3 was compiled in 2003 have pushed the number to 522 species or 708 including subspecies Hybrids Primate hybrids usually arise in captivity but there have also been examples in the wild Hybridization occurs where two species range overlap to form hybrid zones hybrids may be created by humans when animals are placed in zoos or due to environmental pressures such as predation Intergeneric hybridizations hybrids of different genera have also been found in the wild Although they belong to genera that have been distinct for several million years interbreeding still occurs between the gelada and the hamadryas baboon Clones On 24 January 2018 scientists in China reported in the journal Cell the creation of two crab eating macaque clones named Zhong Zhong and Hua Hua using the complex DNA transfer method that produced Dolly the sheep for the first time Anatomy and physiologyHead Primate skulls showing postorbital bar and increasing brain sizes The primate skull has a large domed cranium which is particularly prominent in anthropoids The cranium protects the large brain a distinguishing characteristic of this group The endocranial volume the volume within the skull is three times greater in humans than in the greatest nonhuman primate reflecting a larger brain size The mean endocranial volume is 1 201 cubic centimeters in humans 469 cm3 in gorillas 400 cm3 in chimpanzees and 397 cm3 in orangutans The primary evolutionary trend of primates has been the elaboration of the brain in particular the neocortex a part of the cerebral cortex which is involved with sensory perception generation of motor commands spatial reasoning conscious thought and in humans language While other mammals rely heavily on their sense of smell the arboreal life of primates has led to a tactile visually dominant sensory system a reduction in the olfactory region of the brain and increasingly complex social behavior The visual acuity of humans and other hominids is exceptional they have the most acute vision known among all vertebrates with the exception of certain species of predatory birds Primates have forward facing eyes on the front of the skull binocular vision allows accurate distance perception useful for the brachiating ancestors of all great apes A bony ridge above the eye sockets reinforces weaker bones in the face which are put under strain during chewing Strepsirrhines have a postorbital bar a bone around the eye socket to protect their eyes in contrast the higher primates haplorhines have evolved fully enclosed sockets An 1893 drawing of the hands and feet of various primates Primates show an evolutionary trend towards a reduced snout Technically Old World monkeys are distinguished from New World monkeys by the structure of the nose and from apes by the arrangement of their teeth In New World monkeys the nostrils face sideways in Old World monkeys they face downwards Dental pattern in primates vary considerably although some have lost most of their incisors all retain at least one lower incisor In most strepsirrhines the lower incisors form a toothcomb which is used in grooming and sometimes foraging Old World monkeys have eight premolars compared with 12 in New World monkeys The Old World species are divided into apes and monkeys depending on the number of cusps on their molars monkeys have four apes have five although humans may have four or five The main hominid molar cusp hypocone evolved in early primate history while the cusp of the corresponding primitive lower molar paraconid was lost Prosimians are distinguished by their immobilized upper lips the moist tip of their noses and forward facing lower front teeth Body Vervet hindfoot showing fingerprint ridges on the sole Primates generally have five digits on each limb pentadactyly with a characteristic type of keratin fingernail on the end of each finger and toe The bottom sides of the hands and feet have sensitive pads on the fingertips Most have opposable thumbs a characteristic primate feature most developed in humans though not limited to this order opossums and koalas for example also have them Thumbs allow some species to use tools In primates the combination of opposing thumbs short fingernails rather than claws and long inward closing fingers is a relict of the ancestral practice of gripping branches and has in part allowed some species to develop brachiation swinging by the arms from tree limb to tree limb as a significant means of locomotion Prosimians have clawlike nails on the second toe of each foot called toilet claws which they use for grooming The primate collar bone is a prominent element of the pectoral girdle this allows the shoulder joint broad mobility Compared to Old World monkeys apes have more mobile shoulder joints and arms due to the dorsal position of the scapula broad ribcages that are flatter front to back a shorter less mobile spine and with lower vertebrae greatly reduced resulting in tail loss in some species Prehensile tails are found in the New World atelids including the howler spider woolly spider woolly monkeys and in capuchins Male primates have a low hanging penis and testes descended into a scrotum Sexual dimorphism Distinct sexual size dimorphism can be seen between the male and female gorilla Sexual dimorphism is often exhibited in simians though to a greater degree in Old World species apes and some monkeys than New World species Recent studies involve comparing DNA to examine both the variation in the expression of the dimorphism among primates and the fundamental causes of sexual dimorphism Primates usually have dimorphism in body mass and canine tooth size along with pelage and skin color The dimorphism can be attributed to and affected by different factors including mating system size habitat and diet Comparative analyses have generated a more complete understanding of the relationship between sexual selection natural selection and mating systems in primates Studies have shown that dimorphism is the product of changes in both male and female traits Ontogenetic scaling where relative extension of a common growth trajectory occurs may give some insight into the relationship between sexual dimorphism and growth patterns Some evidence from the fossil record suggests that there was convergent evolution of dimorphism and some extinct hominids probably had greater dimorphism than any living primate Locomotion Diademed sifaka a lemur that is a vertical clinger and leaper Primate species move by brachiation bipedalism leaping arboreal and terrestrial quadrupedalism climbing knuckle walking or by a combination of these methods Several prosimians are primarily vertical clingers and leapers These include many bushbabies all indriids i e sifakas avahis and indris sportive lemurs and all tarsiers Other prosimians are arboreal quadrupeds and climbers Some are also terrestrial quadrupeds while some are leapers Most monkeys are both arboreal and terrestrial quadrupeds and climbers Gibbons muriquis and spider monkeys all brachiate extensively with gibbons sometimes doing so in remarkably acrobatic fashion Woolly monkeys also brachiate at times Orangutans use a similar form of locomotion called quadramanous climbing in which they use their arms and legs to carry their heavy bodies through the trees Chimpanzees and gorillas knuckle walk and can move bipedally for short distances Although numerous species such as australopithecines and early hominids have exhibited fully bipedal locomotion humans are the only extant species with this trait Vision The tapetum lucidum of a northern greater galago typical of prosimians reflects the light of the photographer s flash The evolution of color vision in primates is unique among most eutherian mammals While the remote vertebrate ancestors of the primates possessed three color vision trichromaticism the nocturnal warm blooded mammalian ancestors lost one of three cones in the retina during the Mesozoic era Fish reptiles and birds are therefore trichromatic or tetrachromatic while all mammals with the exception of some primates and marsupials are dichromats or monochromats totally color blind Nocturnal primates such as the night monkeys and bush babies are often monochromatic Catarrhines are routinely trichromatic due to a gene duplication of the red green opsin gene at the base of their lineage 30 to 40 million years ago Platyrrhines on the other hand are trichromatic in a few cases only Specifically individual females must be heterozygous for two alleles of the opsin gene red and green located on the same locus of the X chromosome Males therefore can only be dichromatic while females can be either dichromatic or trichromatic Color vision in strepsirrhines is not as well understood however research indicates a range of color vision similar to that found in platyrrhines Like catarrhines howler monkeys a family of platyrrhines show routine trichromatism that has been traced to an evolutionarily recent gene duplication Howler monkeys are one of the most specialized leaf eaters of the New World monkeys fruits are not a major part of their diets and the type of leaves they prefer to consume young nutritive and digestible are detectable only by a red green signal Field work exploring the dietary preferences of howler monkeys suggests that routine trichromaticism was selected by environment BehaviorSocial systems Richard Wrangham stated that social systems of primates are best classified by the amount of movement by females occurring between groups He proposed four categories Female transfer systems females move away from the group in which they were born Females of a group will not be closely related whereas males will have remained with their natal groups and this close association may be influential in social behavior The groups formed are generally quite small This organization can be seen in chimpanzees where the males who are typically related will cooperate in defense of the group s territory Evidence of this social system has also been found among Neanderthal remains in Spain and in remains of Australopithecus and Paranthropus robustus groups in southern Africa Among New World Monkeys spider monkeys and muriquis use this system A social huddle of ring tailed lemurs The two individuals on the right exposing their white ventral surface are sunning themselves Male transfer systems while the females remain in their natal groups the males will emigrate as adolescents Group sizes are usually larger This system is common among the ring tailed lemur capuchin monkeys and cercopithecine monkeys Monogamous species a male female bond sometimes accompanied by a juvenile offspring There is shared responsibility of parental care and territorial defense The offspring leaves the parents territory during adolescence Indri lariang tarsiers Callitrichidae monkeys and gibbons use this system although monogamy in this context does not necessarily mean absolute sexual fidelity These species do not live in larger groups Solitary species males and females live in overlapping home ranges This type of organization is found in lorises galagos mouse lemurs aye ayes and orangutans Other systems are known to occur as well For example with howler monkeys and gorillas both the males and females typically transfer from their natal group on reaching sexual maturity resulting in groups in which neither the males nor females are typically related Some prosimians colobine monkeys and callitrichid monkeys also use this system The transfer of females or males from their native group is likely an adaptation for avoiding inbreeding An analysis of breeding records of captive primate colonies representing numerous different species indicates that the infant mortality of inbred young is generally higher than that of non inbred young This effect of inbreeding on infant mortality is probably largely a result of increased expression of deleterious recessive alleles see Inbreeding depression Chimpanzees are social great apes Primatologist Jane Goodall who studied in the Gombe Stream National Park noted fission fusion societies in chimpanzees There is fission when the main group splits up to forage during the day then fusion when the group returns at night to sleep as a group This social structure can also be observed in the hamadryas baboon spider monkeys and the bonobo The gelada has a similar social structure in which many smaller groups come together to form temporary herds of up to 600 monkeys Humans also form fission fusion societies In hunter gatherer societies humans form groups which are made up of several individuals that may split up to obtain different resources These social systems are affected by three main ecological factors distribution of resources group size and predation Within a social group there is a balance between cooperation and competition Cooperative behaviors in many primates species include social grooming removing skin parasites and cleaning wounds food sharing and collective defense against predators or of a territory Aggressive behaviors often signal competition for food sleeping sites or mates Aggression is also used in establishing dominance hierarchies In November 2023 scientists reported for the first time evidence that groups of primates particularly bonobos are capable of cooperating with each other Interspecific associations Several species of primates are known to associate in the wild Some of these associations have been extensively studied In the Tai Forest of Africa several species coordinate anti predator behavior These include the Diana monkey Campbell s mona monkey lesser spot nosed monkey western red colobus king colobus western black and white colobus and sooty mangabey which coordinate anti predator alarm calls Among the predators of these monkeys is the common chimpanzee The red tailed monkey associates with several species including the western red colobus blue monkey Wolf s mona monkey mantled guereza black crested mangabey and Allen s swamp monkey Several of these species are preyed upon by the common chimpanzee In South America squirrel monkeys associate with capuchin monkeys This may have more to do with foraging benefits to the squirrel monkeys than anti predation benefits Mating systems Two common marmosets the species lives in monogamous pairsGelada harem one male and multiple females The mating systems of primates vary between monogamy polyandry polygyny and polygynandry In monogamous species adult males and females form long lasting pair bonds Compared to other systems there is little competition for mating rights and males and females tend to be similar in size Polyandry which involves groups consisting of single females mating with multiple males may arise as a secondary mating system in monogamous species In the brown mantled tamarin a female may breeding with one or two males Polyandry may have developed due to the high frequency of twin births which require more help in raising Polygynous species include gorillas Hanuman langurs geladas hamadryas baboons proboscis monkeys and golden snub nosed monkeys and consists of one male mating with multiple females within a harem or one male unit Sexual dimorphism tends to be higher in these species and males may also develop prominent secondary sex characteristics In the patriarchal hamadryas baboon the males aggressively herd females into their groups and violently discipline those that wander By contrast in gelada society which is based on female kinship a male is dependent on the support of the females in his unit and cannot impose on them Polygynous males must defend their harems from rivals who may try to take over In some species such as ring tailed lemurs sifakas macaques most baboons mangabeys squirrel monkeys woolly monkeys spider monkeys woolly spider monkeys chimpanzees and bonobos both males and females mate with multiple partners Polygynandry occurs in multimale multifemale groups and since females mate many times before conception males have large testicles for sperm competition Males may exist in a dominance hierarchy and those at the top will try to monopolize access to the females Consortships may occur in some species but these are short term In solitary living species males and females mate with partners whose home ranges they overlap with This is known as a dispersed mating system Genetic evidence indicates that humans were predominantly polygynous for most of their existence as a species but that this began to shift during the Neolithic when monogamy started becoming widespread concomitantly with the transition from nomadic to sedentary societies Most modern human societies consist of monogamous marriages but allow for polygyny particularly for those of a high status Sexual behavior Bonobos mating Jacksonville Zoo and Gardens Female primates may signal to the male their receptiveness though various displays including eye contact tongue clicking and presenting of the rump Female lemurs lorises and galagos will position themselves in the lordosis pose while female chimpanzees bonobos and some Old World monkeys develop sexual swellings on the rump Copulation in primates typically involves the males mounting the females from behind as with most mammals Belly to belly copulation has been recorded in apes both gibbons and the great apes Human sex positions are modifications of these two positions Primates may egage in sexual activity as part of social bonding including homosexual behaviour Such behavior play an important role in bonobo society in particular female bonobos engage in mutual genital rubbing behavior possibly to bond socially with each other thus forming a female nucleus of bonobo society The bonding among females enables them to dominate most of the males Life history A crab eating macaque breastfeeding her baby Primates have slower rates of development than other mammals All primate infants are breastfed by their mothers with the exception of some human cultures and various zoo raised primates which are fed formula and rely on them for grooming and transportation In some species infants are protected and transported by males in the group particularly males who may be their fathers Other relatives of the infant such as siblings and aunts may participate in its care as well Most primate mothers cease ovulation while breastfeeding an infant once the infant is weaned the mother can reproduce again This often leads to weaning conflict with infants who attempt to continue breastfeeding Infanticide is common in polygynous species such as gray langurs and gorillas Adult males may kill dependent offspring that are not theirs so the female will return to estrus and thus they can sire offspring of their own Social monogamy in some species may have evolved to combat this behavior Polygynandry may also lessen the risk of infanticide since paternity becomes uncertain Primates have a longer juvenile period between weaning and sexual maturity than other mammals of similar size Some primates such as galagos and New World monkeys use tree holes for nesting and park juveniles in leafy patches while foraging Other primates follow a strategy of riding i e carrying individuals on the body while feeding Adults may construct or use nesting sites sometimes accompanied by juveniles for the purpose of resting a behavior which has developed secondarily in the great apes During the juvenile period primates are more susceptible than adults to predation and starvation they gain experience in feeding and avoiding predators during this time They learn social and fighting skills often through playing Primates especially females have longer lifespans than other similarly sized mammals this may be partially due to their slower metabolisms Late in life female catarrhine primates appear to undergo a cessation of reproductive function known as menopause other groups are less studied Diet and feeding Leaf eating mantled guerezaA mouse lemur eating fruit Primates exploit a variety of food sources It has been said that many characteristics of modern primates including humans derive from an early ancestor s practice of taking most of its food from the tropical canopy Most primates include fruit in their diets to obtain easily digested nutrients including carbohydrates and lipids for energy Primates in the suborder Strepsirrhini non tarsier prosimians are able to synthesize vitamin C like most other mammals while primates of the suborder Haplorhini tarsiers monkeys and apes have lost this ability and require the vitamin in their diet Many primates have anatomical specializations that enable them to exploit particular foods such as fruit leaves gum or insects For example leaf eaters such as howler monkeys black and white colobuses and sportive lemurs have extended digestive tracts which enable them to absorb nutrients from leaves that can be difficult to digest Marmosets which are gum eaters have strong incisor teeth enabling them to open tree bark to get to the gum and claws rather than nails enabling them to cling to trees while feeding The aye aye combines rodent like teeth with a long thin middle finger to fill the same ecological niche as a woodpecker It taps on trees to find insect larvae then gnaws holes in the wood and inserts its elongated middle finger to pull the larvae out Some species have additional specializations For example the grey cheeked mangabey has thick enamel on its teeth enabling it to open hard fruits and seeds that other monkeys cannot The gelada is the only primate species that feeds primarily on grass Hunting Humans have traditionally hunted prey for subsistence Tarsiers are the only extant obligate carnivorous primates exclusively eating insects crustaceans small vertebrates and snakes including venomous species Capuchin monkeys can exploit many different types of plant matter including fruit leaves flowers buds nectar and seeds but also eat insects and other invertebrates bird eggs and small vertebrates such as birds lizards squirrels and bats The common chimpanzee eats an omnivorous frugivorous diet It prefers fruit above all other food items and even seeks out and eats them when they are not abundant It also eats leaves and leaf buds seeds blossoms stems pith bark and resin Insects and meat make up a small proportion of their diet estimated as 2 The meat consumption includes predation on other primate species such as the western red colobus monkey The bonobo is an omnivorous frugivore the majority of its diet is fruit but it supplements this with leaves meat from small vertebrates such as anomalures flying squirrels and duikers and invertebrates In some instances bonobos have been shown to consume lower order primates Until the development of agriculture approximately 10 000 years ago Homo sapiens employed a hunter gatherer method as their sole means of food collection This involved combining stationary food sources such as fruits grains tubers and mushrooms insect larvae and aquatic mollusks with wild game which must be hunted and killed in order to be consumed It has been proposed that humans have used fire to prepare and cook food since the time of Homo erectus Around ten thousand years ago humans developed agriculture which substantially altered their diet This change in diet may also have altered human biology with the spread of dairy farming providing a new and rich source of food leading to the evolution of the ability to digest lactose in some adults As prey Predators of primates include various species of carnivorans birds of prey reptiles and other primates Even gorillas have been recorded as prey Predators of primates have diverse hunting strategies and as such primates have evolved several different antipredator adaptations including crypsis alarm calls and mobbing Several species have separate alarm calls for different predators such as air borne or ground dwelling predators Predation may have shaped group size in primates as species exposed to higher predation pressures appear to live in larger groups Communication Indri lemur wailing source source Indri lemur wailing ogg Vorbis format Howler monkey roaring source source track track Howler monkey roaring ogg Vorbis format Vervet monkey alarm call source source Vervet monkey alarm call ogg Vorbis format Siamang singing source source Siamang singing ogg Vorbis format Problems playing these files See media help A pair of black howler monkeys vocalizing Lemurs lorises tarsiers and New World monkeys rely on olfactory signals for many aspects of social and reproductive behavior Specialized glands are used to mark territories with pheromones which are detected by the vomeronasal organ this process forms a large part of the communication behavior of these primates In Old World monkeys and apes this ability is mostly vestigial having regressed as trichromatic eyes evolved to become the main sensory organ Primates also use vocalizations gestures and facial expressions to convey psychological state Facial musculature is very developed in primates particularly in monkeys and apes allowing for complex facial communication Like humans chimpanzees can distinguish the faces of familiar and unfamiliar individuals Hand and arm gestures are also important forms of communication for great apes and a single gesture can have multiple functions Chest beating in male gorillas is a form of visual and non vocal sound communication that serves to show fitness to both rivals and females Primates are a particularly vocal group of mammals Indris and black and white ruffed lemurs make distinctive loud songs and choruses which maintain territories and act as alarm calls The Philippine tarsier has a high frequency limit of auditory sensitivity of approximately 91 kHz with a dominant frequency of 70 kHz among the highest recorded for any terrestrial mammal For Philippine tarsiers these ultrasonic vocalizations might represent a private channel of communication that subverts detection by predators prey and competitors enhances energetic efficiency or improves detection against low frequency background noise Male howler monkeys are among the loudest land mammals as their roars can be heard up to 4 8 km 3 0 mi and relate to intergroup spacing territorial protection and possibly mate guarding Male and female siamangs both possess inflatable pouches in the throat with which pair bonds use to sing duets to each other The vervet monkey gives a distinct alarm call for each of at least four different predators and the reactions of other monkeys vary according to the call Furthermore many primate species including chimpanzees Campbell s mona monkeys or Diana monkeys have been shown to combine vocalizations in sequences suggesting syntax may not be uniquely humans as previously thought but rather evolutionary ancient and its origins may be deeply rooted in the primate lineage Consonant and vowel like sounds exist in some orangutan calls and they maintain their meaning over great distances The time range for the evolution of human language and or its anatomical prerequisites extends at least in principle from the phylogenetic divergence of Homo 2 3 to 2 4 million years ago from Pan 5 to 6 million years ago to the emergence of full behavioral modernity some 50 000 150 000 years ago Few dispute that Australopithecus probably lacked vocal communication significantly more sophisticated than that of great apes in general Intelligence and cognition Primates have advanced cognitive abilities some make tools and use them to acquire food and for social displays some can perform tasks requiring cooperation influence and rank they are status conscious manipulative and capable of deception they can recognise kin and conspecifics and they can learn to use symbols and understand aspects of human language including some relational syntax and concepts of number and numerical sequence Research in primate cognition explores problem solving memory social interaction a theory of mind and numerical spatial and abstract concepts Comparative studies show a trend towards higher intelligence going from prosimians to New World monkeys to Old World monkeys and significantly higher average cognitive abilities in the great apes However there is a great deal of variation in each group e g among New World monkeys both spider and capuchin monkeys have scored highly by some measures as well as in the results of different studies Tool use and manufacture source source source source source source Chimpanzees using twigs to dip for antsCrab eating macaques with stone tools In 1960 Jane Goodall observed a chimpanzee poking pieces of grass into a termite mound and then raising the grass to his mouth After he left Goodall approached the mound and repeated the behaviour because she was unsure what the chimpanzee was doing She found that the termites bit onto the grass with their jaws The chimpanzee had been using the grass as a tool to fish or dip for termites There are more limited reports of the closely related bonobo using tools in the wild it has been claimed they rarely use tools in the wild although they use tools as readily as chimpanzees when in captivity It has been reported that females both chimpanzee and bonobo use tools more avidly than males Orangutans in Borneo scoop catfish out of small ponds Over two years anthropologist Anne Russon observed orangutans learning to jab sticks at catfish to scare them out of the ponds and in to their waiting hands There are few reports of gorillas using tools in the wild An adult female western lowland gorilla used a branch as a walking stick apparently to test water depth and to aid her in crossing a pool of water Another adult female used a detached trunk from a small shrub as a stabilizer during food gathering and another used a log as a bridge The first direct observation of a non ape primate using a tool in a wild environment occurred in 1988 Primatologist Sue Boinski watched an adult male white faced capuchin beat a fer de lance snake to death with a dead branch The black striped capuchin was the first non ape primate for which routine tool use was documented in the wild individuals were observed cracking nuts by placing them on a stone anvil and hitting them with another large stone In Thailand and Myanmar crab eating macaques use stone tools to open nuts oysters and other bivalves and various types of sea snails Chacma baboons use stones as weapons stoning by these baboons is done from the rocky walls of the canyon where they sleep and retreat to when they are threatened Stones are lifted with one hand and dropped over the side whereupon they tumble down the side of the cliff or fall directly to the canyon floor Although they have not been observed to use tools in the wild lemurs in controlled settings have been shown to be capable of understanding the functional properties of the objects they had been trained to use as tools performing as well as tool using haplorhines Soon after her initial discovery of tool use Goodall observed other chimpanzees picking up leafy twigs stripping off the leaves and using the stems to fish for insects This change of a leafy twig into a tool was a major discovery Prior to this scientists thought that only humans manufactured and used tools and that this ability was what separated humans from other animals Chimpanzees have also been observed making sponges out of leaves and moss that suck up water Sumatran orangutans have been observed making and using tools They will break off a tree branch that is about 30 cm long snap off the twigs fray one end and then use the stick to dig in tree holes for termites In the wild mandrills have been observed to clean their ears with modified tools Scientists filmed a large male mandrill at Chester Zoo UK stripping down a twig apparently to make it narrower and then using the modified stick to scrape dirt from underneath its toenails Captive gorillas have made a variety of tools EcologyRhesus macaque at Agra Fort India Non human primates primarily live in the tropical latitudes of Africa Asia and the Americas Species that live outside of the tropics include the Japanese macaque which lives in the Japanese islands of Honshu and Hokkaido the Barbary macaque which lives in North Africa and several species of langur which live in China Primates tend to live in tropical rainforests but are also found in temperate forests savannas deserts mountains and coastal areas The number of primate species within tropical areas has been shown to be positively correlated to the amount of rainfall and the amount of rain forest area Accounting for 25 to 40 of the fruit eating animals by weight within tropical rainforests primates play an important ecological role by dispersing seeds of many tree species Primate habitats span a range of altitudes the black snub nosed monkey has been found living in the Hengduan Mountains at altitudes of 4 700 meters 15 400 ft the mountain gorilla can be found at 4 200 meters 13 200 ft crossing the Virunga Mountains and the gelada has been found at elevations of up to 5 000 m 16 000 ft in the Ethiopian Highlands Some species interact with aquatic environments and may swim or even dive including the proboscis monkey De Brazza s monkey and Allen s swamp monkey Some primates such as the rhesus macaque and gray langurs can exploit human modified environments and even live in cities Interactions between humans and other primatesDisease transmission Close interactions between humans and non human primates NHPs can create pathways for the transmission of zoonotic diseases Viruses such as Herpesviridae most notably Herpes B Virus Poxviridae measles ebola rabies the Marburg virus and viral hepatitis can be transmitted to humans in some cases the viruses produce potentially fatal diseases in both humans and non human primates Legal and social status Slow lorises are popular in the exotic pet trade which threatens wild populations Only humans are recognized as persons and protected in law by the United Nations Universal Declaration of Human Rights The legal status of NHPs on the other hand is the subject of much debate with organizations such as the Great Ape Project GAP campaigning to award at least some of them legal rights In June 2008 Spain became the first country in the world to recognize the rights of some NHPs when its parliament s cross party environmental committee urged the country to comply with GAP s recommendations which are that chimpanzees orangutans and gorillas are not to be used for animal experiments Many species of NHP are kept as pets by humans The Allied Effort to Save Other Primates AESOP estimates that around 15 000 NHPs live as exotic pets in the United States The expanding Chinese middle class has increased demand for NHPs as exotic pets in recent years Although NHP import for the pet trade was banned in the U S in 1975 smuggling still occurs along the United States Mexico border with prices ranging from US 3000 for monkeys to 30 000 for apes Primates are used as model organisms in laboratories and have been used in space missions They serve as service animals for disabled humans Capuchin monkeys can be trained to assist quadriplegic humans their intelligence memory and manual dexterity make them ideal helpers NHPs are kept in zoos around the globe Historically zoos were primarily a form of entertainment but more recently have shifted their focus towards conservation education and research GAP does not insist that all NHPs should be released from zoos primarily because captive born primates lack the knowledge and experience to survive in the wild if released Role in scientific research Sam a rhesus macaque was flown to the edge of space by NASA in the 1959 Little Joe 2 flight of Project Mercury Thousands of non human primates are used around the world in research because of their psychological and physiological similarity to humans In particular the brains and eyes of NHPs more closely parallel human anatomy than those of any other animals NHPs are commonly used in preclinical trials neuroscience ophthalmology studies and toxicity studies Rhesus macaques are often used as are other macaques African green monkeys chimpanzees baboons squirrel monkeys and marmosets both wild caught and purpose bred In 2005 GAP reported that 1 280 of the 3 100 NHPs living in captivity in the United States were used for experiments In 2004 the European Union used around 10 000 NHPs in such experiments in 2005 in Great Britain 4 652 experiments were conducted on 3 115 NHPs Governments of many nations have strict care requirements of NHPs kept in captivity In the US federal guidelines extensively regulate aspects of NHP housing feeding enrichment and breeding European groups such as the European Coalition to End Animal Experiments are seeking a ban on all NHP use in experiments as part of the European Union s review of animal testing legislation Extinction threats Humans are known to hunt other primates for food called bushmeat Pictured are two men who have killed a number of silky sifaka and white headed brown lemurs The International Union for Conservation of Nature IUCN lists more than a third of primates as critically endangered or vulnerable About 60 of primate species are threatened with extinction including 87 of species in Madagascar 73 in Asia 37 in Africa and 36 in South and Central America Additionally 75 of primate species have decreasing populations Trade is regulated as all species are listed by CITES in Appendix II except 50 species and subspecies listed in Appendix I which gain full protection from trade Common threats to primate species include deforestation forest fragmentation monkey drives resulting from primate crop raiding and primate hunting for use in medicines as pets and for food Large scale tropical forest clearing is widely regarded as the process that most threatens primates More than 90 of primate species occur in tropical forests The main cause of forest loss is clearing for agriculture although commercial logging subsistence harvesting of timber mining and dam construction also contribute to tropical forest destruction In Indonesia large areas of lowland forest have been cleared to increase palm oil production and one analysis of satellite imagery concluded that during 1998 and 1999 there was a loss of 1 000 Sumatran orangutans per year in the Leuser Ecosystem alone The critically endangered silky sifaka Primates with a large body size over 5 kg are at increased extinction risk due to their greater profitability to poachers compared to smaller primates They reach sexual maturity later and have a longer period between births Populations therefore recover more slowly after being depleted by poaching or the pet trade Data for some African cities show that half of all protein consumed in urban areas comes from the bushmeat trade Endangered primates such as guenons and the drill are hunted at levels that far exceed sustainable levels This is due to their large body size ease of transport and profitability per animal As farming encroaches on forest habitats primates feed on the crops causing the farmers large economic losses Primate crop raiding gives locals a negative impression of primates hindering conservation efforts Madagascar home to five endemic primate families has experienced the greatest extinction of the recent past since human settlement 1 500 years ago at least eight classes and fifteen of the larger species have become extinct due to hunting and habitat destruction Among the primates wiped out were Archaeoindris a lemur larger than a silverback gorilla and the families Palaeopropithecidae and Archaeolemuridae The critically endangered Sumatran orangutan In Asia Hinduism Buddhism and Islam prohibit eating primate meat however primates are still hunted for food Some smaller traditional religions allow the consumption of primate meat The pet trade and traditional medicine also increase demand for illegal hunting The rhesus macaque a model organism was protected after excessive trapping threatened its numbers in the 1960s the program was so effective that they are now viewed as a pest throughout their range In Central and South America forest fragmentation and hunting are the two main problems for primates Large tracts of forest are now rare in Central America This increases the amount of forest vulnerable to edge effects such as farmland encroachment lower levels of humidity and a change in plant life Movement restriction results in a greater amount of inbreeding which can cause deleterious effects leading to a population bottleneck whereby a significant percentage of the population is lost There are 21 critically endangered primates seven of which have remained on the IUCN s The World s 25 Most Endangered Primates list since the year 2000 the silky sifaka Delacour s langur the white headed langur the gray shanked douc the Tonkin snub nosed monkey the Cross River gorilla and the Sumatran orangutan Miss Waldron s red colobus was recently declared extinct when no trace of the subspecies could be found from 1993 to 1999 A few hunters have found and killed individuals since then but the subspecies prospects remain bleak See alsoAnimals portalMammals portalPrimates portalArboreal theory Great Ape Project Human evolution International Primate Day List of primates List of fossil primates Monkey Day PrimatologyFootnotesAlthough the monophyletic relationship between lemurs and lorisoids is widely accepted their clade name is not The term lemuriform is used here because it derives from one popular taxonomy that clumps the clade of toothcombed primates into one infraorder and the extinct non toothcombed adapiforms into another both within the suborder Strepsirrhini However another popular alternative taxonomy places the lorisoids in their own infraorder Lorisiformes Article 6 Everyone has the right to recognition everywhere as a person before the law ReferencesGroves C P 2005 Wilson D E Reeder D M eds Mammal Species of the World A Taxonomic and Geographic Reference 3rd ed Baltimore Johns Hopkins University Press pp 111 184 ISBN 0 801 88221 4 OCLC 62265494 Silcox Mary T Bloch Jonathan I Boyer Doug M Chester Stephen G B Lopez Torres Sergi 2017 The evolutionary radiation of plesiadapiforms Evolutionary Anthropology Issues News and Reviews 26 2 74 94 doi 10 1002 evan 21526 ISSN 1520 6505 PMID 28429568 Primate Merriam Webster Online Dictionary Merriam Webster Retrieved 2008 07 21 The Book of Popular Science 1963 p 257 Chisholm Hugh ed 1911 Ape Encyclopaedia Britannica Vol 02 11th ed Cambridge University Press p 160 Weisberger Mindy March 23 2024 Why don t humans have tails Scientists find answers in an unlikely place CNN Archived from the original on March 24 2024 Retrieved March 24 2024 Dixson A F 1981 The Natural History of the Gorilla London Weidenfeld amp Nicolson ISBN 978 0 297 77895 0 Definitions of paraphyly vary for the one used here see e g Stace Clive A 2010 Classification by molecules What s in it for field botanists PDF Watsonia 28 103 122 archived from the original PDF on 2011 07 26 retrieved 2010 02 07 Definitions of monophyly vary for the one used here see e g Mishler Brent D 2009 Species are not Uniquely Real Biological Entities in Ayala F J amp Arp R eds Contemporary Debates in Philosophy of Biology pp 110 122 doi 10 1002 9781444314922 ch6 ISBN 978 1 4443 1492 2 Benton 2005 p 371 Benton 2005 pp 378 380 Linnaeus C 1758 Sistema naturae per regna tria Naturae secundum classes ordines genera species cum characteribus differentiis synonimis locis Tomus I Impensis direct Laurentii Salvii Holmia pp 20 32 Linnaeus C 1735 Sistema naturae sive regna tria Naturae systematice proposita per classes ordines genera amp species apud Theodorum Haak Lugduni Batavorum pp s p Blainville H 1839 Nouvelle classification des Mammiferes Annales Francaises et Etrangeres d Anatomie et de Physiologie Appliquees a la Medicine et a l Histoire Naturelle 3 pp 268 269 Thorington R W amp Anderson S 1984 Primates In Anderson S amp Jones J K eds Orders and Families of Recent Mammals of the World New York John Wiley and Sons pp 187 217 ISBN 978 0 471 08493 8 McKenna M C amp Bell S K 1997 Classification of Mammals Above the species level New York Columbia University Press p 631 ISBN 0 231 11013 8 Strier K 2007 Primate Behavioral Ecology Third ed Pearson Allyn and Bacon pp 50 53 ISBN 978 0 205 44432 8 Cartmill M Smith F H 2011 The Human Lineage John Wiley amp Sons ISBN 978 1 118 21145 8 Groves C P 2001 Primate Taxonomy Smithsonian Institution Press ISBN 1 56098 872 X Szalay amp Delson 1980 p 149 Cartmill 2010 p 15 Hartwig 2011 pp 20 21 Janecka J E Miller W Pringle T H Wiens F Zitzmann A Helgen K M Springer M S Murphy W J 2 November 2007 Molecular and Genomic Data Identify the Closest Living Relative of Primates Science 318 5851 792 794 Bibcode 2007Sci 318 792J doi 10 1126 science 1147555 PMID 17975064 S2CID 12251814 Kavanagh M 1983 A Complete Guide to Monkeys Apes and Other Primates New York Viking Press pp 18 ISBN 0 670 43543 0 McKenna M C amp Bell S K 1997 Classification of Mammals Above the Species Level New York Columbia University Press p 329 ISBN 0 231 11012 X Williams B A Kay R F Kirk E C 2010 New perspectives on anthropoid origins Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 107 11 4797 4804 Bibcode 2010PNAS 107 4797W doi 10 1073 pnas 0908320107 PMC 2841917 PMID 20212104 Stanyon Roscoe Springer Mark S Meredith Robert W Gatesy John Emerling Christopher A Park Jong Rabosky Daniel L Stadler Tanja Steiner Cynthia Ryder Oliver A Janecka Jan E Fisher Colleen A Murphy William J 2012 Macroevolutionary Dynamics and Historical Biogeography of Primate Diversification Inferred from a Species Supermatrix PLOS ONE 7 11 e49521 Bibcode 2012PLoSO 749521S doi 10 1371 journal pone 0049521 ISSN 1932 6203 PMC 3500307 PMID 23166696 Jameson Natalie M Hou Zhuo Cheng Sterner Kirstin N Weckle Amy Goodman Morris Steiper Michael E Wildman Derek E September 2011 Genomic data reject the hypothesis of a prosimian primate clade Journal of Human Evolution 61 3 295 305 Bibcode 2011JHumE 61 295J doi 10 1016 j jhevol 2011 04 004 ISSN 0047 2484 PMID 21620437 Pozzi Luca Hodgson Jason A Burrell Andrew S Sterner Kirstin N Raaum Ryan L Disotell Todd R June 2014 Primate phylogenetic relationships and divergence dates inferred from complete mitochondrial genomes Molecular Phylogenetics and Evolution 75 165 183 Bibcode 2014MolPE 75 165P doi 10 1016 j ympev 2014 02 023 ISSN 1055 7903 PMC 4059600 PMID 24583291 Stanyon Roscoe Finstermeier Knut Zinner Dietmar Brameier Markus Meyer Matthias Kreuz Eva Hofreiter Michael Roos Christian 16 July 2013 A Mitogenomic Phylogeny of Living Primates PLOS ONE 8 7 e69504 Bibcode 2013PLoSO 869504F doi 10 1371 journal pone 0069504 ISSN 1932 6203 PMC 3713065 PMID 23874967 O Leary M A et al 8 February 2013 The placental mammal ancestor and the post K Pg radiation of placentals Science 339 6120 662 667 Bibcode 2013Sci 339 662O doi 10 1126 science 1229237 hdl 11336 7302 PMID 23393258 S2CID 206544776 Wilson Mantilla Gregory P Chester Stephen G B Clemens William A Moore Jason R Sprain Courtney J Hovatter Brody T Mitchell William S Mans Wade W Mundil Roland Renne Paul R 2021 Earliest Palaeocene purgatoriids and the initial radiation of stem primates Royal Society Open Science 8 2 210050 Bibcode 2021RSOS 810050W doi 10 1098 rsos 210050 PMC 8074693 PMID 33972886 Williams B A Kay R F Kirk E C 2010 New perspectives on anthropoid origins Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 107 11 4797 4804 Bibcode 2010PNAS 107 4797W doi 10 1073 pnas 0908320107 PMC 2841917 PMID 20212104 Miller E R Gunnell G F Martin R D 2005 Deep Time and the Search for Anthropoid Origins PDF American Journal of Physical Anthropology 128 60 95 doi 10 1002 ajpa 20352 PMID 16369958 Chatterjee Helen J Ho Simon Y W Barnes Ian Groves Colin 27 October 2009 Estimating the phylogeny and divergence times of primates using a supermatrix approach BMC Evolutionary Biology 9 1 259 Bibcode 2009BMCEE 9 259C doi 10 1186 1471 2148 9 259 PMC 2774700 PMID 19860891 Lee M September 1999 Molecular Clock Calibrations and Metazoan Divergence Dates Journal of Molecular Evolution 49 3 385 391 Bibcode 1999JMolE 49 385L doi 10 1007 PL00006562 PMID 10473780 S2CID 1629316 Scientists Push Back Primate Origins From 65 Million To 85 Million Years Ago Science Daily Retrieved 2008 10 24 Tavare S Marshall C R Will O Soligo C Martin R D April 18 2002 Using the fossil record to estimate the age of the last common ancestor of extant primates Nature 416 6882 726 729 Bibcode 2002Natur 416 726T doi 10 1038 416726a PMID 11961552 S2CID 4368374 Klonisch T Froehlich C Tetens F Fischer B Hombach Klonisch S 2001 Molecular Remodeling of Members of the Relaxin Family During Primate Evolution Molecular Biology and Evolution 18 3 393 403 doi 10 1093 oxfordjournals molbev a003815 PMID 11230540 Horvath J et al 2008 Development and Application of a Phylogenomic Toolkit Resolving the Evolutionary History of Madagascar s Lemurs Genome Research 18 3 489 499 doi 10 1101 gr 7265208 PMC 2259113 PMID 18245770 Mittermeier R Ganzhorn J Konstant W Glander K Tattersall I Groves C Rylands A Hapke A Ratsimbazafy J Mayor M Louis E Rumpler Y Schwitzer C Rasoloarison R December 2008 Lemur Diversity in Madagascar International Journal of Primatology 29 6 1607 1656 doi 10 1007 s10764 008 9317 y S2CID 17614597 Archived from the original PDF on 2021 02 15 Retrieved 2019 09 24 Sellers Bill 2000 10 20 Primate Evolution PDF University of Edinburgh pp 13 17 Archived from the original PDF on 2008 10 29 Retrieved 2008 10 23 Hartwig W 2007 Primate Evolution In Campbell C Fuentes A MacKinnon K Panger M Bearder S eds Primates in Perspective Oxford University Press pp 13 17 ISBN 978 0 19 517133 4 Williams B A Kay R F Christopher Kirk E Ross C F 2010 Darwinius masillae is a strepsirrhine a reply to Franzen et al 2009 PDF Journal of Human Evolution 59 5 567 573 discussion 573 9 Bibcode 2010JHumE 59 567W doi 10 1016 j jhevol 2010 01 003 PMID 20188396 Archived from the original PDF on 2013 05 17 Retrieved 2015 09 04 Ciochon R amp Fleagle J 1987 Primate Evolution and Human Origins Menlo Park California Benjamin Cummings p 72 ISBN 978 0 202 01175 2 Garbutt N 2007 Mammals of Madagascar A Complete Guide A amp C Black Publishers pp 85 86 ISBN 978 0 300 12550 4 Mittermeier R A et al 2006 Lemurs of Madagascar 2nd ed Conservation International pp 23 26 ISBN 1 881173 88 7 Shekelle M 2005 Evolutionary Biology of Tarsiers Archived from the original on 2008 09 07 Retrieved 2008 08 22 Schmidt T et al 3 May 2005 Rapid electrostatic evolution at the binding site for cytochrome c on cytochrome c oxidase in anthropoid primates Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 102 18 6379 6384 Bibcode 2005PNAS 102 6379S doi 10 1073 pnas 0409714102 PMC 1088365 PMID 15851671 Wade Lizzie June 5 2013 Early Primate Weighed Less Than an Ounce ScienceNow Archived from the original on 2013 06 08 Retrieved 2013 06 07 Kay R F 2012 Evidence for an Asian origin of stem anthropoid s Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 109 26 10132 10133 Bibcode 2012PNAS 10910132K doi 10 1073 pnas 1207933109 PMC 3387095 PMID 22699505 Chaimanee Y Chavasseau O Beard K C Kyaw A A Soe A N Sein C Lazzari V Marivaux L Marandat B Swe M Rugbumrung M Lwin T Valentin X Zin Maung Maung Thein Jaeger J J 2012 Late Middle Eocene primate from Myanmar and the initial anthropoid colonization of Africa Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 109 26 10293 10297 Bibcode 2012PNAS 10910293C doi 10 1073 pnas 1200644109 PMC 3387043 PMID 22665790 Marivaux L et al 2005 06 14 Anthropoid primates from the Oligocene of Pakistan Bugti Hills Data on early anthropoid evolution and biogeography Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 102 24 8436 8441 Bibcode 2005PNAS 102 8436M doi 10 1073 pnas 0503469102 PMC 1150860 PMID 15937103 Schrago C G amp Russo C A M 2003 Timing the Origin of New World Monkeys PDF Reprint Molecular Biology and Evolution 20 10 1620 1625 doi 10 1093 molbev msg172 PMID 12832653 Houle A 1999 The origin of platyrrhines An evaluation of the Antarctic scenario and the floating island model American Journal of Physical Anthropology 109 4 541 559 doi 10 1002 SICI 1096 8644 199908 109 4 lt 541 AID AJPA9 gt 3 0 CO 2 N PMID 10423268 Andrews P amp Kelley J 2007 Middle Miocene Dispersals of Apes Folia Primatologica 78 5 6 328 343 doi 10 1159 000105148 PMID 17855786 S2CID 19293586 Strier K 2007 Primate Behavioral Ecology 3rd ed Allyn amp Bacon pp 7 64 71 77 182 185 273 280 284 287 298 ISBN 978 0 205 44432 8 Pough F W Janis C M Heiser J B 2005 1979 Primate Evolution and the Emergence of Humans Vertebrate Life 7th ed Pearson pp 650 ISBN 0 13 127836 3 IUCN SSC Primate Specialist Group 1 March 2021 Primate diversity by region International Union for the Conservation of Nature Tenaza R 1984 Songs of hybrid gibbons Hylobates lar H muelleri American Journal of Primatology 8 3 249 253 doi 10 1002 ajp 1350080307 PMID 31986810 S2CID 84957700 Bernsteil I S 1966 Naturally occurring primate hybrid Science 154 3756 1559 1560 Bibcode 1966Sci 154 1559B doi 10 1126 science 154 3756 1559 PMID 4958933 S2CID 85898043 Sugawara K January 1979 Sociological study of a wild group of hybrid baboons between Papio anubis and P hamadryas in the Awash Valley Ethiopia Primates 20 1 21 56 doi 10 1007 BF02373827 S2CID 23061688 Jolly C J Woolley Barker Tamsin et al 1997 Intergeneric Hybrid Baboons International Journal of Primatology 18 4 597 627 doi 10 1023 A 1026367307470 S2CID 27900830 Liu Zhen et al 24 January 2018 Cloning of Macaque Monkeys by Somatic Cell Nuclear Transfer Cell 172 4 881 887 e7 doi 10 1016 j cell 2018 01 020 PMID 29395327 Normile Dennis 24 January 2018 These monkey twins are the first primate clones made by the method that developed Dolly Science doi 10 1126 science aat1066 Retrieved 24 January 2018 Cyranoski David 24 January 2018 First monkeys cloned with technique that made Dolly the sheep Chinese scientists create cloned primates that could revolutionize studies of human disease Nature 553 7689 387 388 Bibcode 2018Natur 553 387C doi 10 1038 d41586 018 01027 z PMID 29368720 Briggs Helen 24 January 2018 First monkey clones created in Chinese laboratory BBC News Retrieved 24 January 2018 Scientists Successfully Clone Monkeys Are Humans Up Next The New York Times Associated Press 24 January 2018 Retrieved 24 January 2018 Pough F W Janis C M Heiser J B 2005 1979 Characteristics of Primates Vertebrate Life 7th ed Pearson pp 630 ISBN 0 13 127836 3 Aiello L amp Dean C 1990 An Introduction to Human Evolutionary Anatomy Academic Press pp 193 ISBN 0 12 045590 0 Primate Encyclopaedia Britannica Online Encyclopaedia Britannica Inc 2008 Retrieved 2008 07 21 Myers P 1999 Primates On line Animal Diversity Web Retrieved 2008 06 03 Caves Eleanor M May 2018 Visual Acuity and the Evolution of Signals Trends in Ecology amp Evolution 33 5 358 372 Bibcode 2018TEcoE 33 358C doi 10 1016 j tree 2018 03 001 PMID 29609907 Retrieved 29 July 2018 Kirk E Christopher Kay Richard F 2004 Ross Callum F Kay Richard F eds The Evolution of High Visual Acuity in the Anthropoidea Anthropoid Origins New Visions Developments in Primatology Progress and Prospects Boston MA Springer US pp 539 602 doi 10 1007 978 1 4419 8873 7 20 ISBN 978 1 4419 8873 7 retrieved 2023 07 30 Campbell B G amp Loy J D 2000 Humankind Emerging 8th ed Allyn amp Bacon p 85 ISBN 0 673 52364 0 White T amp Kazlev A 2006 01 08 Archonta Primates Palaeos Archived from the original on 2008 05 12 Retrieved 2008 06 03 Macdonald David 2006 Primates The Encyclopedia of Mammals The Brown Reference Group plc pp 282 307 ISBN 0 681 45659 0 Ash M M Nelson S J Wheeler R C 2003 Wheeler s Dental Anatomy Physiology and Occlusion W B Saunders p 12 ISBN 978 0 7216 9382 8 Garber PA Rehg JA November 1999 The ecological role of the prehensile tail in white faced capuchins Cebus capucinus American Journal of Physical Anthropology 110 3 325 39 doi 10 1002 SICI 1096 8644 199911 110 3 lt 325 AID AJPA5 gt 3 0 CO 2 D PMID 10516564 Russo GA Young JW November 2011 Tail growth tracks the ontogeny of prehensile tail use in capuchin monkeys Cebus albifrons and C apella American Journal of Physical Anthropology 146 3 465 73 doi 10 1002 ajpa 21617 PMID 21953012 Friderun Ankel Simons 27 July 2010 Primate Anatomy An Introduction Academic Press pp 442 521 ISBN 978 0 08 046911 9 Ralls K 1976 Mammals in Which Females are Larger Than Males The Quarterly Review of Biology 51 2 245 76 doi 10 1086 409310 PMID 785524 S2CID 25927323 Lindstedtand amp Boyce Boyce Mark S July 1985 Seasonality Fasting Endurance and Body Size in Mammals The American Naturalist 125 6 873 Bibcode 1985ANat 125 873L doi 10 1086 284385 S2CID 84308684 Frisch J E 1963 Sex differences in the canines of the gibbon Hylobates lar Primates 4 2 1 10 doi 10 1007 BF01659148 S2CID 189798134 Kay R F 1975 The functional adaptations of primate molar teeth American Journal of Physical Anthropology 43 2 195 215 doi 10 1002 ajpa 1330430207 PMID 810034 Crook J H 1972 Sexual selection dimorphism and social organization in the primates In Campbell B G ed Sexual selection and the descent of man Aldine Transaction pp 246 ISBN 978 0 202 02005 1 Cheverud J M Dow M M Leutenegger W November 1985 The quantitative assessment of phylogenetic constraints in comparative analyses Sexual dimorphism in body weight among primates Evolution 39 6 1335 1351 doi 10 2307 2408790 JSTOR 2408790 PMID 28564267 Leutenegger W Cheverud J M 1982 Correlates of sexual dimorphism in primates Ecological and size variables International Journal of Primatology 3 4 387 402 doi 10 1007 BF02693740 S2CID 38220186 Plavcan J M 2001 Sexual dimorphism in primate evolution American Journal of Physical Anthropology 33 25 53 doi 10 1002 ajpa 10011 PMID 11786990 S2CID 31722173 O Higgins P Collard M 2002 Sexual dimorphism and facial growth in papionine monkeys Journal of Zoology 257 2 255 72 doi 10 1017 S0952836902000857 Sussman R W 1999 Primate Ecology and Social Structure Volume 1 Lorises Lemurs and Tarsiers Needham Heights MA Pearson Custom Publishing amp Prentice Hall pp 78 89 90 108 121 123 233 ISBN 0 536 02256 9 Sussman R W 2003 Primate Ecology and Social Structure Volume 2 New World Monkeys Revised First ed Needham Heights MA Pearson Custom Publishing amp Prentice Hall pp 77 80 132 133 141 143 ISBN 0 536 74364 9 Glazier S D Flowerday C A 2003 Selected Readings in the Anthropology of Religion Theoretical and Methodological Essays Greenwood Publishing Group pp 53 ISBN 9780313300905 Arrese C A Oddy Alison Y et al 2005 Cone topography and spectral sensitivity in two potentially trichromatic marsupials the quokka Setonix brachyurus and quenda Isoodon obesulus Proceedings of the Royal Society B 272 1565 791 6 doi 10 1098 rspb 2004 3009 PMC 1599861 PMID 15888411 Bowmaker J K Astell S Hunt D M Mollon J D 1991 Photosensitive and photostable pigments in the retinae of Old World monkeys PDF The Journal of Experimental Biology 156 1 1 19 Bibcode 1991JExpB 156 1B doi 10 1242 jeb 156 1 1 ISSN 0022 0949 PMID 2051127 Retrieved 2008 06 16 Surridge A K amp D Osorio 2003 Evolution and selection of trichromatic vision in primates Trends in Ecology and Evolution 18 4 198 205 doi 10 1016 S0169 5347 03 00012 0 Lucas P W Dominy N J Riba Hernandez P Stoner K E Yamashita N Loria Calderon E Petersen Pereira W Rojas Duran Y Salas Pena R Solis Madrigal S Osorio D Darvell B W 2003 Evolution and function of routine trichromatic vision in primates Evolution 57 11 2636 43 doi 10 1554 03 168 PMID 14686538 S2CID 739130 Wrangham R W 1982 Mutualism kinship and social evolution Current Problems in Sociobiology Cambridge University Press pp 269 89 ISBN 0 521 24203 7 Goldberg T L Wrangham R W September 1997 Genetic correlates of social behavior in wild chimpanzees evidence from mitochondrial DNA Animal Behaviour 54 3 559 70 doi 10 1006 anbe 1996 0450 PMID 9299041 S2CID 18223362 Bowdler Neil 21 December 2010 Neanderthal family found cannibalised in cave in Spain BBC News Bowdler Neil 2 June 2011 Ancient cave women left childhood homes BBC News Retrieved 2 June 2011 Copeland Sandi R et al 1 June 2011 Strontium isotope evidence for landscape use by early hominins Nature 474 7349 76 78 doi 10 1038 nature10149 PMID 21637256 S2CID 205225222 Fiore A D amp Campbell C J 2007 The Atelines In Campbell C J Fuentes A MacKinnon K C Panger M amp Bearder S K eds Primates in Perspective Oxford University Press p 175 ISBN 978 0 19 517133 4 Bartlett T Q 2007 The Hylobatidae In Campbell C J Fuentes A MacKinnon K C Panger M Bearder S K eds Primates in Perspective Oxford University Press p 283 ISBN 978 0 19 517133 4 Dixon Alan F 2012 Primate Sexuality Oxford University Press pp 32 61 ISBN 9780199544646 Watts D P 1996 Comparative socio ecology of gorillas In McGrew W C Marchant L F Nishida T eds Great Ape Societies Cambridge England Cambridge Univ Press pp 16 28 ISBN 978 0521555364 Charpentier MJ Widdig A Alberts SC December 2007 Inbreeding depression in non human primates a historical review of methods used and empirical data American Journal of Primatology 69 12 1370 86 doi 10 1002 ajp 20445 PMID 17486606 S2CID 46626761 Ralls K Ballou J 1982 Effect of inbreeding on infant mortality in captive primates PDF International Journal of Primatology 3 4 491 505 doi 10 1007 BF02693747 S2CID 10954608 permanent dead link Constable J L Ashley M V Goodall J Pusey A E May 2001 Noninvasive paternity assignment in Gombe chimpanzees Molecular Ecology 10 5 1279 300 Bibcode 2001MolEc 10 1279C doi 10 1046 j 1365 294X 2001 01262 x PMID 11380884 S2CID 46604532 Rowe N 1996 The Pictorial Guide to the Living Primates Pogonias Press pp 4 139 143 15 185 223 ISBN 0 9648825 0 7 Couzin Iain D Laidre Mark E August 2009 Fission fusion populations Current Biology 19 15 R633 R635 Bibcode 2009CBio 19 R633C doi 10 1016 j cub 2009 05 034 ISSN 0960 9822 PMID 19674541 S2CID 13549970 Pough F W Janis C M Heiser J B 2005 1979 Primate Societies Vertebrate Life 7th ed Pearson pp 621 623 ISBN 0 13 127836 3 Smuts B B Cheney D L Seyfarth R M Wrangham R W amp Struhsaker T T Eds 1987 Primate Societies Chicago University of Chicago Press for articles on the structure and function of various primate societies Zimmer Carl 16 November 2023 Scientists Find First Evidence That Groups of Apes Cooperate Some bonobos are challenging the notion that humans are the only primates capable of group to group alliances The New York Times Archived from the original on 16 November 2023 Retrieved 17 November 2023 Samuni Liran et al 16 November 2023 Cooperation across social borders in bonobos Science 382 6672 805 809 Bibcode 2023Sci 382 805S doi 10 1126 science adg0844 PMID 37972165 Archived from the original on 17 November 2023 Retrieved 17 November 2023 Shultz S amp Thomsett S 2007 Interactions between African Crowned Eagles and Their Prey Community In McGraw W Zuberbuhler K amp Noe R eds Monkeys of Tai Forest An African Primate Community Cambridge University Press p 181 ISBN 978 0 521 81633 5 Bshary R 2007 Interactions between Red Colobus Monkeys and Chimpanzees In McGraw W Zuberbuhler K Noe R eds Monkeys of Tai Forest An African Primate Community Cambridge University Press pp 155 170 ISBN 978 0 521 81633 5 Stanford C 1998 Chimpanzee and Red Colobus the ecology of predator and prey Harvard University Press pp 130 138 233 ISBN 0 674 00722 0 Boinski S 2000 Social Manipulation Within and Between Troops Mediates Primate Group Movement In Boinski S Garber P eds On the Move how and why animals travel in groups University of Chicago Press pp 447 448 ISBN 0 226 06340 2 Dupanloup Isabelle Pereira Luisa Bertorelle Giorgio Calafell Francesc Prata Maria Joao Amorim Antonio Barbujani Guido 1 July 2003 A Recent Shift from Polygyny to Monogamy in Humans Is Suggested by the Analysis of Worldwide Y Chromosome Diversity Journal of Molecular Evolution 57 1 85 97 Bibcode 2003JMolE 57 85D doi 10 1007 s00239 003 2458 x ISSN 0022 2844 PMID 12962309 Retrieved 13 July 2024 via Springer Link Dixon Alan F 2012 Primate Sexuality Oxford University Press pp 130 131 135 149 156 200 202 ISBN 9780199544646 de Waal FB March 1995 Bonobo sex and society PDF Scientific American 272 3 82 8 Bibcode 1995SciAm 272c 82W doi 10 1038 scientificamerican0395 82 PMID 7871411 Archived from the original PDF on 27 January 2012 Retrieved 21 December 2011 Opie Christopher Atkinson Quentin D Dunbarc Robin I M Shultz Susanne 2013 Male infanticide leads to social monogamy in primates Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 110 33 13328 13332 Bibcode 2013PNAS 11013328O doi 10 1073 pnas 1307903110 PMC 3746880 PMID 23898180 De Ruiter Jan R Van Hooff Jan A R A M amp Scheffrahn Wolfgang 1994 Social and genetic aspects of paternity in wild long tailed macaques Macaca fascicularis Behaviour 129 3 4 203 24 doi 10 1163 156853994x00613 JSTOR 4535195 Kappeler Peter M 1998 Nests Tree Holes and the Evolution of Primate Life Histories American Journal of Primatology 46 1 7 33 doi 10 1002 SICI 1098 2345 1998 46 1 lt 7 AID AJP3 gt 3 0 CO 2 PMID 9730211 S2CID 196589387 Ross Caroline 1991 Park or ride Evolution of infant carrying in primates International Journal of Primatology 22 5 Kluwer Academic Publishing 749 771 doi 10 1023 A 1012065332758 S2CID 25301078 Mintz Zoe 14 January 2014 Humans And Primates Burn 50 Percent Fewer Calories Each Day Than Other Mammals www ibtimes com IBT Media Inc Retrieved 2014 01 14 Walker ML Herndon JG Herndon 2008 Menopause in nonhuman primates Biology of Reproduction 79 3 398 406 doi 10 1095 biolreprod 108 068536 PMC 2553520 PMID 18495681 Milton K 1993 Diet and Primate Evolution PDF Scientific American Vol 269 no 2 pp 86 93 Bibcode 1993SciAm 269b 86M doi 10 1038 scientificamerican0893 86 PMID 8351513 Pollock J I Mullin R J 1986 Vitamin C biosynthesis in prosimians Evidence for the anthropoid affinity of Tarsius American Journal of Physical Anthropology 73 1 65 70 doi 10 1002 ajpa 1330730106 PMID 3113259 Archived from the original on 2012 06 28 Retrieved 2010 03 16 Milliken G W Ward J P Erickson C J 1991 Independent digit control in foraging by the aye aye Daubentonia madagascariensis Folia Primatologica 56 4 219 224 doi 10 1159 000156551 PMID 1937286 Hiller C 2000 Theropithecus gelada Animal Diversity Web Retrieved 2008 08 08 Wright P Simmons E Gursky S 2003 Introduction In Wright P Simmons E Gursky S eds Tarsiers Past Present and Future Rutgers University Press p 1 ISBN 0 8135 3236 1 Goodall Jane 1986 The Chimpanzees of Gombe Patterns of Behavior Belknap Press of Harvard University Press ISBN 0 674 11649 6 Guernsey Paul WHAT DO CHIMPS EAT All About Wildlife Archived from the original on 2019 11 18 Retrieved 2013 04 22 Ihobe H 1992 Observations on the meat eating behavior of wild bonobos Pan paniscus at Wamba Republic of Zaire Primates 33 2 247 250 doi 10 1007 BF02382754 S2CID 10063791 Rafert J Vineberg E O 1997 Bonobo Nutrition relation of captive diet to wild diet PDF Bonobo Husbandry Manual American Association of Zoos and Aquariums Archived from the original PDF on 2012 04 25 Surbeck M Fowler A Deimel C Hohmann G 2008 Evidence for the consumption of arboreal diurnal primates by bonobos Pan paniscus American Journal of Primatology 71 2 171 4 doi 10 1002 ajp 20634 PMID 19058132 S2CID 32622605 Surbeck M Hohmann G Hohmann 14 October 2008 Primate hunting by bonobos at LuiKotale Salonga National Park Current Biology 18 19 R906 7 Bibcode 2008CBio 18 R906S doi 10 1016 j cub 2008 08 040 PMID 18957233 S2CID 6708310 Cordain L Eaton SB Sebastian A et al February 2005 Origins and evolution of the Western diet health implications for the 21st century Am J Clin Nutr 81 2 341 54 doi 10 1093 ajcn 81 2 341 PMID 15699220 Ulijaszek SJ November 2002 Human eating behaviour in an evolutionary ecological context Proc Nutr Soc 61 4 517 26 doi 10 1079 PNS2002180 PMID 12691181 Earliest agriculture in the Americas Archived 3 June 2010 at the Wayback Machine Earliest cultivation of barley Archived 16 February 2007 at the Wayback Machine Earliest cultivation of figs Archived 2 June 2006 at the Wayback Machine retrieved 19 February 2007 Krebs JR September 2009 The gourmet ape evolution and human food preferences Am J Clin Nutr 90 3 707S 11S doi 10 3945 ajcn 2009 27462B PMID 19656837 Holden C Mace R October 1997 Phylogenetic analysis of the evolution of lactose digestion in adults Hum Biol 69 5 605 28 PMID 9299882 Fichtel Claudia 2012 Predation In Mitani John C Call Josep Kappeler Peter M Palombit Ryne A Silk Joan B eds The Evolution of Primate Societies University of Chicago Press pp 169 84 ISBN 978 0 226 53172 4 Liman E R Innan H 2003 Relaxed selective pressure on an essential component of pheromone transduction in primate evolution PDF Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 100 6 3328 3332 Bibcode 2003PNAS 100 3328L doi 10 1073 pnas 0636123100 PMC 152292 PMID 12631698 Retrieved 2008 07 23 Egnor R Miller C Hauser M D 2004 Nonhuman Primate Communication PDF Encyclopedia of Language and Linguistics 2nd ed Elsevier ISBN 0 08 044299 4 Archived from the original PDF on 2008 09 10 Pollick A S de Waal F B M 2007 Ape gestures and language evolution Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 104 19 8184 8189 Bibcode 2007PNAS 104 8184P doi 10 1073 pnas 0702624104 PMC 1876592 PMID 17470779 Burrows A M 2008 The facial expression musculature in primates and its evolutionary significance BioEssays 30 3 212 225 doi 10 1002 bies 20719 PMID 18293360 S2CID 205478149 Wright E Grawunder S Ndayishimiye E Galbany J McFarlin S C Stoinski T S Robbins M M 2021 Chest beats as an honest signal of body size in male mountain gorillas Gorilla beringei beringei Scientific Reports 11 1 6879 Bibcode 2021NatSR 11 6879W doi 10 1038 s41598 021 86261 8 PMC 8032651 PMID 33833252 Geissmann Thomas amp Mutschler Thomas 2006 Diurnal Distribution of Loud Calls in Sympatric Wild Indris Indri indri and Ruffed Lemurs Varecia variegata Implications for Call Functions PDF Primates Journal of Primatology 47 4 393 6 doi 10 1007 s10329 006 0189 5 PMID 16736264 S2CID 1586657 Ramsier M A Cunningham A J Moritz G L Finneran J J Williams C V Ong P S Gursky Doyen S L Dominy N J 2012 Primate communication in the pure ultrasound Biology Letters 8 4 508 511 doi 10 1098 rsbl 2011 1149 PMC 3391437 PMID 22319094 da Cunha R G T Byrne R 2006 Roars of Black Howler Monkeys Alouatta caraya Evidence for a Function in Inter Group Spacing Behaviour 143 10 1169 1199 doi 10 1163 156853906778691568 JSTOR 4536401 Black howler monkey Smithsonian s National Zoo amp Conservation Biology Institute 25 April 2016 Retrieved 2016 07 10 Haimoff E H 1983 Brief report Occurrence of anti resonance in the song of the siamang Hylobates syndactylus American Journal of Primatology 5 3 249 256 doi 10 1002 ajp 1350050309 PMID 31986856 S2CID 85262432 Seyfarth R M Cheney D L Marler Peter 1980 Vervet Monkey Alarm Calls Semantic communication in a Free Ranging Primate Animal Behaviour 28 4 1070 1094 doi 10 1016 S0003 3472 80 80097 2 S2CID 53165940 Leroux M Schel A M Wilke C Chandia B Zuberbuhler K Slocombe K E Townsend S 2023 Call combinations and compositional processing in wild chimpanzees Nature Communications 14 1 2225 Bibcode 2023NatCo 14 2225L doi 10 1038 s41467 023 37816 y PMC 10160036 PMID 37142584 Coye C Ouattara K Zuberbuhler K Lemasson A 2015 Suffixation influences receivers behaviour in non human primates Proceedings of the Royal Society B 282 1807 20150265 doi 10 1098 rspb 2015 0265 PMC 4424650 PMID 25925101 Coye C Zuberbuhler K Lemasson A 2016 Morphologically structured vocalizations in female Diana monkeys Animal Behaviour 115 97 105 doi 10 1016 j anbehav 2016 03 010 hdl 10023 10629 Leroux M Townsend S 2020 Call Combinations in Great Apes and the Evolution of Syntax Animal Behavior and Cognition 7 2 131 139 doi 10 26451 abc 07 02 07 2020 Lameira A R et al 2021 Orangutan information broadcast via consonant like and vowel like calls breaches mathematical models of linguistic evolution Biology Letters 17 9 doi 10 1098 rsbl 2021 0302 PMC 8478518 PMID 34582737 Arcadi AC Aug 2000 Vocal responsiveness in male wild chimpanzees implications for the evolution of language J Hum Evol 39 2 205 23 Bibcode 2000JHumE 39 205A doi 10 1006 jhev 2000 0415 PMID 10968929 S2CID 7403772 Boesch C Boesch H 1990 Tool Use and Tool Making in Wild Chimpanzees Folia Primatologica 54 1 2 86 99 doi 10 1159 000156428 PMID 2157651 Westergaard G C Lundquist A L et al 1998 Why some capuchin monkeys Cebus apella use probing tools and others do not Journal of Comparative Psychology 112 2 207 211 doi 10 1037 0735 7036 112 2 207 PMID 9642788 de Waal F B M Davis J M 2003 Capuchin cognitive ecology cooperation based on projected returns Neuropsychologia 41 2 221 228 doi 10 1016 S0028 3932 02 00152 5 PMID 12459220 S2CID 8190458 Paar L A Winslow J T Hopkins W D de Waal F B M 2000 Recognizing facial cues Individual discrimination by chimpanzees Pan troglodytes and rhesus monkeys Macaca mulatta Journal of Comparative Psychology 114 1 47 60 doi 10 1037 0735 7036 114 1 47 PMC 2018744 PMID 10739311 Byrne Richard Corp Nadia 2004 Neocortex size predicts deception rate in primates Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B Biological Sciences 271 1549 1693 1699 doi 10 1098 rspb 2004 2780 PMC 1691785 PMID 15306289 Paar L A de Waal F B M 1999 Visual kin recognition in chimpanzees Nature 399 6737 647 648 Bibcode 1999Natur 399 647P doi 10 1038 21345 PMID 10385114 S2CID 4424086 Fujita K Watanabe K Widarto T H Suryobroto B 1997 Discrimination of macaques by macaques The case of sulawesi species Primates 38 3 233 245 doi 10 1007 BF02381612 S2CID 21042762 Call J 2001 Object permanence in orangutans Pongo pygmaeus chimpanzees Pan troglodytes and children Homo sapiens Journal of Comparative Psychology 115 2 159 171 doi 10 1037 0735 7036 115 2 159 PMID 11459163 Itakura S Tanaka M June 1998 Use of experimenter given cues during object choice tasks by chimpanzees Pan troglodytes an orangutan Pongo pygmaeus and human infants Homo sapiens Journal of Comparative Psychology 112 2 119 126 doi 10 1037 0735 7036 112 2 119 PMID 9642782 Gouteux S Thinus Blanc C Vauclair J 2001 Rhesus monkeys use geometric and nongeometric information during a reorientation task PDF Journal of Experimental Psychology General 130 3 505 519 doi 10 1037 0096 3445 130 3 505 PMID 11561924 Tomasello M amp Call J 1997 Primate Cognition Oxford University Press US ISBN 978 0 19 510624 4 Deaner R O van Schaik C P Johnson V E 2006 Do some taxa have better domain general cognition than others A metaanalysis of nonhuman primate studies Evolutionary Psychology 4 149 196 doi 10 1177 147470490600400114 S2CID 16702785 Reader S M Hager Y Laland K N 2011 The evolution of primate general and cultural intelligence PDF Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 366 1567 1017 1027 doi 10 1098 rstb 2010 0342 PMC 3049098 PMID 21357224 Archived from the original PDF on 2011 10 03 Retrieved 2011 07 04 Toolmaking The Jane Goodall Institute Retrieved 2013 08 01 Bonobos ApeTag 2010 Archived from the original on 2013 11 02 Retrieved 2013 08 03 Gruber T Clay Z Zuberbuhler K 2010 A comparison of bonobo and chimpanzee tool use evidence for a female bias in the Pan lineage PDF Animal Behaviour 80 6 1023 1033 doi 10 1016 j anbehav 2010 09 005 S2CID 14923158 Bower B 18 April 2011 Orangutans use simple tools to catch fish Wired Retrieved 2013 08 05 Breuer T Ndoundou Hockemba M Fishlock V 2005 First observation of tool use in wild gorillas PLOS Biology 3 11 e380 doi 10 1371 journal pbio 0030380 PMC 1236726 PMID 16187795 Boinski S 1988 Use of a club by a wild white faced capuchin Cebus capucinus to attack a venomous snake Bothrops asper American Journal of Primatology 14 2 177 179 doi 10 1002 ajp 1350140208 PMID 31973450 S2CID 84653622 Fragaszy D Izar P Visalberghi E Ottoni E B de Oliveira M G 2004 Wild capuchin monkeys Cebus libidinosus use anvils and stone pounding tools American Journal of Primatology 64 4 359 366 doi 10 1002 ajp 20085 PMID 15580579 S2CID 16222308 Gumert M D Kluck M Malaivijitnond S 2009 The physical characteristics and usage patterns of stone axe and pounding hammers used by long tailed macaques in the Andaman Sea region of Thailand American Journal of Primatology 71 7 594 608 doi 10 1002 ajp 20694 PMID 19405083 S2CID 22384150 Hamilton W J Buskirk R E Buskirk W H 1975 Defensive stoning by baboons Nature 256 5517 488 489 Bibcode 1975Natur 256 488H doi 10 1038 256488a0 S2CID 4149862 Fichtel C Kappeler P M 2010 Chapter 19 Human universals and primate symplesiomorphies Establishing the lemur baseline In Kappeler P M Silk J B eds Mind the Gap Tracing the Origins of Human Universals Springer ISBN 978 3 642 02724 6 Sugiyama Y 1995 Drinking tools of wild chimpanzees at Bossou American Journal of Primatology 37 1 263 269 doi 10 1002 ajp 1350370308 PMID 31936951 S2CID 86473603 Sumatran orangutans OrangutanIslands com Archived from the original on 2013 11 26 Retrieved 2013 08 02 van Schaik C Fox E Sitompul A 1996 Manufacture and use of tools in wild Sumatran orangutans Naturwissenschaften 83 4 186 188 Bibcode 1996NW 83 186V doi 10 1007 BF01143062 PMID 8643126 S2CID 27180148 Gill Victoria 22 July 2011 Mandrill monkey makes pedicuring tool BBC Retrieved 2013 08 11 Vancatova M 2008 Gorillas and Tools Part I Retrieved 2013 08 04

rec-icon Recommended Topics
Share this article
Read the free encyclopedia and learn everything...
See more
Read the free encyclopedia. All information in Wikipedia is available. No payment required.
Share this article on
Share
XXX 0C
Saturday, 08 February, 2025
Follow Us On