This article includes a list of references, related reading, or external links, but its sources remain unclear because it lacks inline citations.(February 2010) |
In linguistics, productivity is the degree to which speakers of a language use a particular grammatical process, especially in word formation. It compares grammatical processes that are in frequent use to less frequently used ones that tend towards lexicalization. Generally the test of productivity concerns identifying which grammatical forms would be used in the coining of new words: these will tend to only be converted to other forms using productive processes.
Examples in English
In standard English, the formation of preterite and past participle forms of verbs by means of ablaut (as Germanic strong verbs, for example, sing–sang–sung) is no longer considered productive. Newly coined verbs in English overwhelmingly use the 'weak' (regular) ending -ed for the past tense and past participle (for example, spammed, e-mailed). Similarly, the only clearly productive plural ending is -(e)s; it is found on the vast majority of English count nouns and is used to form the plurals of neologisms, such as FAQs and Muggles. The ending -en, on the other hand, is no longer productive, being found only in oxen, children, and the now-rare brethren (as a plural of brother). Because these old forms can sound incorrect to modern ears, regularization can wear away at them until they are no longer used: brethren has now been replaced with the more regular-sounding brothers except when talking about religious orders. It appears that many strong verbs were completely lost during the transition from Old English to Middle English, possibly because they sounded archaic or were simply no longer truly understood.
In both cases, however, occasional exceptions have occurred. A false analogy with other verbs caused dug to become thought of as the 'correct' preterite and past participle form of dig (the King James Bible preferred digged in 1611) and more recent examples, like snuck from sneak and dove from dive, have similarly become popular. Some American English dialects also use the non-standard drug as the past tense of drag.
Significance
Since production of novel (new, non-established) structures is the clearest proof of usage of a grammatical process, the evidence most often appealed to as establishing productivity is the appearance of novel forms of the type the process leads one to expect, and many people would limit the definition offered above to exclude use of a grammatical process that does not result in a novel structure. Thus in practice, and, for many, in theory, productivity is the degree to which speakers use a particular grammatical process for the formation of novel structures. A productive grammatical process defines an open class, one which admits new words or forms. Non-productive grammatical processes may be seen as operative within closed classes: they remain within the language and may include very common words, but are not added to and may be lost in time or through regularization converting them into what now seems to be a correct form.
Productivity is, as stated above and implied in the examples already discussed, a matter of degree, and there are a number of areas in which that may be shown to be true. As the modern example of snuck from sneak shows, what has apparently been non-productive for many decades or even centuries may suddenly come to some degree of productive life, and it may do so in certain dialects or sociolects while not in others, or in certain parts of the vocabulary but not others. Some patterns are only very rarely productive, others may be used by a typical speaker several times a year or month, whereas others (especially syntactic processes) may be used productively dozens or hundreds of times in a typical day. It is not atypical for more than one pattern with similar functions to be comparably productive, to the point that a speaker can be in a quandary as to which form to use —e.g., would it be better to say that a taste or color like that of raisins is raisinish, raisiny, raisinlike, or even raisinly?
It can also be very difficult to assess when a given usage is productive or when a person is using a form that has already been learned as a whole. Suppose a reader comes across an unknown word such as despisement meaning "an attitude of despising". The reader may apply the verb+ment noun-formational process to understand the word perfectly well, and this would be a kind of productive use. This would be essentially independent of whether or not the writer had also used the same process productively in coining the term, or whether he or she had learned the form from previous usage (as most English speakers have learned government, for instance), and no longer needed to apply the process productively in order to use the word. Similarly a speaker or writer's use of words like raisinish or raisiny may or may not involve productive application of the noun+ish and noun+y rules, and the same is true of a hearer or reader's understanding of them. But it will not necessarily be at all clear to an outside observer, or even to the speaker and hearer themselves, whether the form was already learnt and whether the rules were applied or not.
Examples in other languages
One study, which focuses on the usage of the Dutch suffix -heid (comparable to -ness in English) hypothesizes that -heid gives rise to two kinds of abstract nouns: those referring to concepts and those referring to states of affairs. It shows that the referential function of -heid is typical for the lowest-frequency words, while its conceptual function is typical for the highest-frequency words. It claims that high-frequency formations with the suffix -heid are available in the mental lexicon, whereas low-frequency words and neologisms are produced and understood by rule.
See also
References
- Baayen, Harald. (1992). Quantitative aspects of morphological productivity. In G. Booij & J. van Marle (Eds.), Yearbook of morphology, 1991. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers, 109–149. ISBN 0-7923-1416-6.
- Baayen, Harald & Rochelle Lieber. (1991). Productivity and English derivation: A corpus-based study. Linguistics 29, 801-844.
- Bauer, Laurie. (2001). Morphological productivity. Cambridge studies in linguistics (No. 95). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 0-521-79238-X.
- Bolozky, Shmuel. (1999). Measuring productivity in word formation. Leiden: Brill. ISBN 90-04-11252-9.
- Hay, Jennifer & Harald Baayen. (2002). Parsing and productivity. In G. Booij & J. van Marle (Eds.), Yearbook of morphology, 2002, 203–35. Dordrecht: Kluwer.
- Palmer, Chris C. (2015). Measuring productivity diachronically: nominal suffixes in English letters, 1400–1600. English Language and Linguistics, 19, 107-129. doi:10.1017/S1360674314000264.
- Plag, Ingo. (1999). Morphological productivity: Structural constraints in English derivation. Topics in English linguistics (No. 28). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. ISBN 3-11-015833-7.
- Säily, Tanja. (2014). Sociolinguistic variation in English derivational productivity: Studies and methods in diachronic corpus linguistics. Helsinki: Société Néophilologique.
- Schröder, Anne. (2011). On the productivity of verbal prefixation in English: Synchronic and diachronic perspectives. Tübingen: Narr.
- Trips, Carola. (2009). Lexical semantics and diachronic morphology: The development of -hood, -dom and -ship in the history of English. Tübingen: Niemeyer.
Notes
- BAAYEN, R. & NEIJT, A. (2009). Productivity in context: a case study of a Dutch suffix. Linguistics, 35(3), pp. 565-588. Retrieved 24 Oct. 2017, from doi:10.1515/ling.1997.35.3.565
This article includes a list of references related reading or external links but its sources remain unclear because it lacks inline citations Please help improve this article by introducing more precise citations February 2010 Learn how and when to remove this message In linguistics productivity is the degree to which speakers of a language use a particular grammatical process especially in word formation It compares grammatical processes that are in frequent use to less frequently used ones that tend towards lexicalization Generally the test of productivity concerns identifying which grammatical forms would be used in the coining of new words these will tend to only be converted to other forms using productive processes Examples in EnglishIn standard English the formation of preterite and past participle forms of verbs by means of ablaut as Germanic strong verbs for example sing sang sung is no longer considered productive Newly coined verbs in English overwhelmingly use the weak regular ending ed for the past tense and past participle for example spammed e mailed Similarly the only clearly productive plural ending is e s it is found on the vast majority of English count nouns and is used to form the plurals of neologisms such as FAQs and Muggles The ending en on the other hand is no longer productive being found only in oxen children and the now rare brethren as a plural of brother Because these old forms can sound incorrect to modern ears regularization can wear away at them until they are no longer used brethren has now been replaced with the more regular sounding brothers except when talking about religious orders It appears that many strong verbs were completely lost during the transition from Old English to Middle English possibly because they sounded archaic or were simply no longer truly understood In both cases however occasional exceptions have occurred A false analogy with other verbs caused dug to become thought of as the correct preterite and past participle form of dig the King James Bible preferred digged in 1611 and more recent examples like snuck from sneak and dove from dive have similarly become popular Some American English dialects also use the non standard drug as the past tense of drag SignificanceSince production of novel new non established structures is the clearest proof of usage of a grammatical process the evidence most often appealed to as establishing productivity is the appearance of novel forms of the type the process leads one to expect and many people would limit the definition offered above to exclude use of a grammatical process that does not result in a novel structure Thus in practice and for many in theory productivity is the degree to which speakers use a particular grammatical process for the formation of novel structures A productive grammatical process defines an open class one which admits new words or forms Non productive grammatical processes may be seen as operative within closed classes they remain within the language and may include very common words but are not added to and may be lost in time or through regularization converting them into what now seems to be a correct form Productivity is as stated above and implied in the examples already discussed a matter of degree and there are a number of areas in which that may be shown to be true As the modern example of snuck from sneak shows what has apparently been non productive for many decades or even centuries may suddenly come to some degree of productive life and it may do so in certain dialects or sociolects while not in others or in certain parts of the vocabulary but not others Some patterns are only very rarely productive others may be used by a typical speaker several times a year or month whereas others especially syntactic processes may be used productively dozens or hundreds of times in a typical day It is not atypical for more than one pattern with similar functions to be comparably productive to the point that a speaker can be in a quandary as to which form to use e g would it be better to say that a taste or color like that of raisins is raisinish raisiny raisinlike or even raisinly It can also be very difficult to assess when a given usage is productive or when a person is using a form that has already been learned as a whole Suppose a reader comes across an unknown word such as despisement meaning an attitude of despising The reader may apply the verb ment noun formational process to understand the word perfectly well and this would be a kind of productive use This would be essentially independent of whether or not the writer had also used the same process productively in coining the term or whether he or she had learned the form from previous usage as most English speakers have learned government for instance and no longer needed to apply the process productively in order to use the word Similarly a speaker or writer s use of words like raisinish or raisiny may or may not involve productive application of the noun ish and noun y rules and the same is true of a hearer or reader s understanding of them But it will not necessarily be at all clear to an outside observer or even to the speaker and hearer themselves whether the form was already learnt and whether the rules were applied or not Examples in other languagesOne study which focuses on the usage of the Dutch suffix heid comparable to ness in English hypothesizes that heid gives rise to two kinds of abstract nouns those referring to concepts and those referring to states of affairs It shows that the referential function of heid is typical for the lowest frequency words while its conceptual function is typical for the highest frequency words It claims that high frequency formations with the suffix heid are available in the mental lexicon whereas low frequency words and neologisms are produced and understood by rule See alsoWord formation InflectionReferencesBaayen Harald 1992 Quantitative aspects of morphological productivity In G Booij amp J van Marle Eds Yearbook of morphology 1991 Dordrecht Kluwer Academic Publishers 109 149 ISBN 0 7923 1416 6 Baayen Harald amp Rochelle Lieber 1991 Productivity and English derivation A corpus based study Linguistics 29 801 844 Bauer Laurie 2001 Morphological productivity Cambridge studies in linguistics No 95 Cambridge Cambridge University Press ISBN 0 521 79238 X Bolozky Shmuel 1999 Measuring productivity in word formation Leiden Brill ISBN 90 04 11252 9 Hay Jennifer amp Harald Baayen 2002 Parsing and productivity In G Booij amp J van Marle Eds Yearbook of morphology 2002 203 35 Dordrecht Kluwer Palmer Chris C 2015 Measuring productivity diachronically nominal suffixes in English letters 1400 1600 English Language and Linguistics 19 107 129 doi 10 1017 S1360674314000264 Plag Ingo 1999 Morphological productivity Structural constraints in English derivation Topics in English linguistics No 28 Berlin Mouton de Gruyter ISBN 3 11 015833 7 Saily Tanja 2014 Sociolinguistic variation in English derivational productivity Studies and methods in diachronic corpus linguistics Helsinki Societe Neophilologique Schroder Anne 2011 On the productivity of verbal prefixation in English Synchronic and diachronic perspectives Tubingen Narr Trips Carola 2009 Lexical semantics and diachronic morphology The development of hood domand shipin the history of English Tubingen Niemeyer NotesBAAYEN R amp NEIJT A 2009 Productivity in context a case study of a Dutch suffix Linguistics 35 3 pp 565 588 Retrieved 24 Oct 2017 from doi 10 1515 ling 1997 35 3 565