The English modal auxiliary verbs are a subset of the English auxiliary verbs used mostly to express modality, properties such as possibility and obligation. They can most easily be distinguished from other verbs by their defectiveness (they do not have participles or plain forms) and by their lack of the ending ‑(e)s for the third-person singular.[3]
The central English modal auxiliary verbs are can (with could), may (with might), shall (with should), will (with would), and must. A few other verbs are usually also classed as modals: ought, and (in certain uses) dare, and need. Use (/jus/, rhyming with "loose") is included as well. Other expressions, notably had better, share some of their characteristics.
Modal auxiliary verbs distinguished grammatically
A list of what tend to be regarded as modal auxiliary verbs in Modern English, along with their inflected forms, is shown in the following table.
Contractions are shown only if their orthography is distinctive. There are also unstressed versions that are typically, although not necessarily, written in the standard way. Where there is a blank, the modal auxiliary verb lacks this form. (A corresponding lexical verb may have the form. For example, although the lexical verb need has a preterite form, the modal auxiliary verb need does not.)
Citation form | Plain present | Preterite | Confusible lexical homonym? | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Neutral | Contr. | Negative | Neutral | Contr. | Negative | ||
will | will | 'll | won't | would | 'd | wouldn't | none |
may | may | might | mightn't | none | |||
can | can | can't, cannot | could | couldn't | none | ||
shall | shall | 'll | shan't | should | shouldn't | none | |
must | must | mustn't | none | ||||
ought | ought | oughtn't | marginal ('art' in some varieties[citation needed]) | ||||
need | need | needn't | exists | ||||
dare | dare | daren't | dared | exists | |||
had better | had better | 'd better, ∅ better | hadn't better | none | |||
used | used | usedn't | exists |
Criteria for modal auxiliary verbs
Descriptive grammars of English differ slightly on the criteria they set for modal auxiliary verbs. According to The Cambridge Grammar of the English Language, the criteria are as follows.
Auxiliary verbs
Modal auxiliary verbs are a subset of auxiliary verbs and thus meet the criteria for these. For lists of those criteria, see the article English auxiliary verbs, but among them are that the verbs (i) can invert with their subjects (notably in questions, Must I go?), (ii) can be negated with not (I must not go; Must I not go?), and (iii) have negative inflected forms (won't, wouldn't).
No untensed forms
To illustrate untensed forms, those of the irregular lexical verb take and the non-modal auxiliary verb be are the plain take and be (as in Take it!, I didn't take it, and Don't be silly), the gerund-participles taking and being, and the past participles taken and been.
Modal auxiliary verbs lack untensed forms. Attempting to use them brings ungrammatical results:
- *I will can drive if I take ten lessons.
- *Canning drive would be helpful.
- *I have could/canned drive since I was 18.
Compare the grammaticality of non-modal auxiliary verb be in I will be able to drive, being able to drive, and I have been able to drive.
No subject–verb agreement
This refers to agreement of a verb (in present tense) with its third-person singular subject:
- She can/*cans try.
Compare lexical verb try in She tries/*try, and non-modal auxiliary verb do in She does/*do try.
Had better and (as an auxiliary verb) used lack present tense forms. Other than in the present tense, even lexical verbs lack subject agreement and so this test is inapplicable to either had better or used.
Only a bare infinitival clause as complement
Whereas the lexical verb seem takes a to-infinitival clause (It seemed to happen), and the non-modal auxiliary verb have takes a past participial clause complement (It has happened), a modal auxiliary verb can, in principle, take only a bare infinitival clause (a subordinate clause with the plain form of the verb without to) as its complement:
- It can be a surprise.
- *It can to be a surprise.
- *It can being a surprise.
If they are modal auxiliary verbs, then ought and used are exceptions to this (although ought is increasingly used with a bare infinitival clause complement).
Bare infinitival clause complements are not unique to modal auxiliary verbs. Do is a non-modal auxiliary verb that takes one (Did you move the piano?); help is a lexical verb that can do so (I helped move the piano).
Ability to occur in remote apodosis
An apodosis is the "then" half of a conditional statement. (The "if" half is the protasis.) Remote here means "thought by the speaker to be unlikely" or "known by the speaker to be untrue".
- If I were an elephant, I would eat more apples.
Compare lexical verb eat in *If I were an elephant, I ate more apples, and non-modal auxiliary verb be in *If I were an elephant, I was able to eat more apples.
Must satisfies this only for a minority of speakers, and it is questionable whether had better does so.
The Cambridge Grammar comments on may that:
here there is evidence that for some speakers may and might have diverged to the extent that they are no longer inflectional forms of a single lexeme, but belong to distinct lexemes, may and might, each of which – like must – lacks a preterite....
Used does not satisfy this.
Preterite usable in the main clause for modal remoteness
- I could drive there, I suppose.
If similarly intended (as a doubtful or incredulous contemplation of an option for the future), attempts at this with a lexical or non-modal auxiliary verb are ungrammatical: *I drove there, I suppose; *I was going to drive there, I suppose.
Other than when used for backshift, should has diverged in meaning so far from shall be usable here only with difficulty. As they lack preterite forms, must, ought and need cannot be used in this way, and so that criterion does not apply to them. And used describes the past, not the present or future.
Comments
The following verbs, shown in present–preterite pairs, satisfy or come close to satisfying all of the above criteria and can be classed as the central modal verbs of English:
- can (with could)
- will (with would)
- may (with might) – although the lack in today's Standard English of a negative present inflection (*mayn't) means that it fails one of the criteria for auxiliary verbs
- shall (with should) – although the semantic divergence of shall and should means that its success with one criterion is debatable
- must – although its lack of a preterite (see its etymology below) means that it neither passes nor fails one of the criteria
Even for lexical verbs, preterite forms have uses besides referring to the past, but for modal auxiliary verbs, such uses are particularly important: (Could you pass me the sauce?; Without my phone I might easily be lost; You should work harder; I would avoid that street).
Ought, dare, need, and used satisfy some of the criteria above, and are more (ought, dare, need) or less (used) often categorized as modal verbs.[15]Had better is sometimes called a modal idiom.
Other English auxiliary verbs appear in a variety of different forms and are not regarded as modal verbs:
- be, used as an auxiliary verb in passive voice, continuous aspect and indeed in virtually all of its uses, even as a copula;
- have, used as an auxiliary verb in perfect aspect constructions and the idiom have got (to); it is also used in have to, which has modal meaning, but here (as when denoting possession (very broadly understood)) have only rarely follows auxiliary verb syntax;
- do, see do-support;
- to, of to-infinitival clauses (if to is a defective verb, not a subordinator).
Lists of modal auxiliary verbs
Five recent scholarly descriptions of verbs disagree among themselves on the extension of modal auxiliary verb: on which verbs are modal auxiliary verbs.
They agree that can (with could), may (with might), must, shall (with should) and will (with would) are, or are among, the "central modal auxiliaries" (A Comprehensive Grammar of the English Language, 1985), "secondary or modal auxiliaries" (F. R. Palmer, 1988), "modal auxiliaries" (Anthony R. Warner, 1993), "central members of the modal auxiliary class" (The Cambridge Grammar of the English Language, 2002), or "core modal verbs" (Bas Aarts, 2011).
Among these five verbs, The Cambridge Grammar selects the pair can and will (with could and would) as "the most straightforward of the modal auxiliaries". Peter Collins agrees.
All five accord ought, need and dare a less clear or merely a marginal membership.
A Comprehensive Grammar and Warner do likewise for use; the other three deny that it is a modal auxiliary verb. For that reason, it is discussed primarily not in this article but in English auxiliary verbs.)
As for would in would rather, would sooner and would as soon, and have in had better, had best and had rather, only The Cambridge Grammar notes all six, but each of the other four descriptions of auxiliary verbs notes three or more. Of the three to six idioms that each discussion notes, there is no variation in the status that it accords to them. Warner calls the three that he notes (would rather, had better, had rather) modal auxiliaries. Palmer says that the same three are not modal auxiliaries. Both A Comprehensive Grammar and Aarts use the term modal idiom for a choice of five.The Cambridge Grammar sees modal characteristics in all six uses of these two auxiliary verbs.
A Comprehensive Grammar calls both have got (I've got to go now) and be to (You are to hand over the cash) modal idioms. None of the other descriptions agrees.
Palmer calls be bound/able/going/willing to and have (got) to semi-modals.A Comprehensive Grammar calls be able/about/apt/
Etymology
The modals can and could are from Old English can(n) and cuþ, which were respectively the present and preterite forms of the verb cunnan ("be able"). The silent l in the spelling of could results from analogy with would and should.
Similarly, may and might are from Old English mæg and meahte, respectively the present and preterite forms of magan ("may, to be able"); shall and should are from sceal and sceolde, respectively the present and preterite forms of sculan ("owe, be obliged"); and will and would are from wille and wolde, respectively the present and preterite forms of willan ("wish, want").
The aforementioned Old English verbs cunnan, magan, sculan, and willan followed the preterite-present paradigm (or, in the case of willan, a similar but irregular paradigm), which explains the absence of the ending -s in the third-person present forms can, may, shall, and will. (The original Old English forms given above were first- and third-person singular forms; their descendant forms have become generalized to all persons and numbers.)
The verb must comes from Old English moste, part of the verb motan ("be able/obliged (to do something)"). This was another preterite-present verb, of which moste was in fact the preterite (the present form mot gave rise to mote, which was used as a modal verb in Early Modern English, but must has now lost its past connotations and has replaced mote). Similarly, ought was originally a past form—it derives from ahte, preterite of agan ("own"), another Old English preterite-present verb whose present tense form, ah, has also given the modern (regular) verb owe, and ought was formerly used as a preterite form of owe.
The verb dare also originates from a preterite-present verb, durran ("dare"), specifically its present tense dear(r) although in its non-modal uses in Modern English, it is conjugated regularly. However, need comes from the regular Old English verb neodian (meaning "be necessary")—the alternative third person form need (in place of needs), which has become the norm in modal uses, became common in the 16th century.
Preterite forms
The preterite forms given above (could, might, should, and would, corresponding to can, may, shall, and will, respectively) do not always simply modify the meaning of the modal to give it past reference. The only one regularly used as an ordinary past tense is could when referring to ability: I could swim may serve as a past form of I can swim.
All the preterites are used as past equivalents for the corresponding present modals in indirect speech and similar clauses requiring the rules of sequence of tenses to be applied. For example, if it were said in 1960 that People think that we will all be driving hovercars by the year 2000, it might now be reported that In 1960, people thought we would all be driving hovercars by the year 2000.
This "future-in-the-past" (also known as the past prospective) use of would can also occur in a main clause: I moved to Green Gables in 1930; I would live there for the next ten years.
In many cases, in order to give modals past reference, they are used together with the auxiliary have and a past participle, as in I should have asked her; You may have seen me. Sometimes these expressions are limited in meaning; for example, must have can refer only to certainty, whereas past obligation is expressed by an alternative phrase such as had to (see § Replacements for defective forms below).
Conditional sentences
The preterite forms of modals are used in the apodosis (then-clause) of counterfactual conditional sentences. The modal would (or should as a first-person alternative) is used to produce the conditional construction typically used in clauses of this type: If you loved me, you would support me. It can be replaced by could (meaning "would be able (to do something)") and might (meaning "would possibly") as appropriate.
When the clause has past reference, the construction with the modal plus have (see above) is used: If they (had) wanted to do it, they would (could/might) have done it by now. (The would have done construction is called the conditional perfect.)
The protasis (if-clause) of such a sentence typically contains the preterite form of a verb (or the past perfect construction, for past reference), without any modal. The modal could may be used here in its role of the preterite form of can (if I could speak French). However, all the modal preterites can be used in such clauses with certain types of hypothetical future reference: if I should lose or should I lose (equivalent to if I lose); if you would/might/could stop doing that (usually used as a form of request).
Sentences with the verb wish and expressions of wish using if only... follow similar patterns to the if-clauses that are referred to above when they have counterfactual present or past reference. When they express a desired event in the near future, the modal would is used: I wish you would visit me; If only he would give me a sign.
For more information see English conditional sentences.
Second-person singular forms
Early Modern English often distinguished between second-person plural you (or ye) and second-person singular thou. Rather as English verbs other than modal auxiliaries agree with third-person singular subjects in today's English, Early Modern English verbs in general (modal auxiliaries included) agreed with a second-person subject that was distinctively singular. (There was no such agreement with instances of you or ye that happened to have singular reference.) Examples from Shakespeare are shown below.
Plain present | Preterite | |
---|---|---|
can | canst | couldst |
dare | darest | durst |
may | mayst, mayest | mightst |
must | must | |
need | needest | |
ought | oughtest | |
shall | shalt | shouldst |
will | wilt | wouldst |
- Thou canst not then be false to any man. (Hamlet, I.3)
- Couldst thou save nothing? (King Lear, III.4)
- Arrest me, foolish fellow, if thou darest. (Comedy of Errors, IV.1)
- Durst thou have look'd upon him being awake, / And hast thou kill'd him sleeping? (Midsummer Night's Dream, III.2)
- Safe mayst thou wander, safe return again! (Cymbeline, III.5); If thou mayest discern by that which is left of him what he is, fetch me to the sight of him. (Winter's Tale, III.3)
- Then mightst thou speak, then mightst thou tear thy hair (Romeo and Juliet, III.3)
- thou must know the king is full of grief (Winter's Tale, IV.4)
- thy horse stands behind the hedge: when thou needest him, there thou shalt find him (Henry IV, Part 1, II.2)
- thou oughtest not to let thy horse wear a cloak (Henry VI, Part 2, IV.7)
- Wherefore shouldst thou pity her? (Two Gentlemen of Verona, IV.4)
- Wilt thou be lord of all the world? (Antony and Cleopatra, II.7)
- Why, whither, Adam, wouldst thou have me go? (As You Like It, II.3)
Replacements for defective forms
As noted above, English modal verbs are defective in that they do not have any untensed form, or, for some, preterite form. However, in many cases, expressions can carry the same meaning as the modal and be used to supply the missing forms:
- The modals can and could, expressing ability, can be replaced by be able to, with the appropriate inflection of be.
- The modals may and might, expressing permission, can be replaced by be allowed to, again with the appropriate inflection of be.
- The modal must in most meanings can be replaced by have to, with the appropriate inflection of have.
- When used for futurity, will can be replaced by be going to, with the appropriate inflection of be.
- The modals should and ought to might be replaced by be supposed to, again with the appropriate inflection of be.
Weak forms
Most of the modals have negative inflected forms: can't, won't, etc. Although they started as weak forms (contractions), they are no longer so. Genuine contractions are:
- can /ˈkæn/ → /kən/
- could /ˈkʊd/ → /kəd/
- shall /ˈʃæl/ → /ʃəl/
- should /ˈʃʊd/ → /ʃəd/
- will /ˈwɪl/ → /wəl/, /l/
- would /ˈwʊd/ → /wəd/, /d/
- had better /ˈhæd ˈbɛtəɹ/ → /həd ˈbɛtəɹ/, /d ˈbɛtəɹ/, /ˈbɛtəɹ/
When shall and should are first-person replacements for will and would, they too may take the weak forms ‑ll and ‑d.[citation needed]
A combination like should have is normally reduced to /ʃʊd(h)əv/ or just /ʃʊdə/ shoulda. Also, ought to can become /ɔːtə/ oughta. See weak and strong forms in English.
Effect of negation
Either or both of two kinds of negation can apply to a construction using a modal auxiliary verb. What is called internal negation is limited to the subordinate clause. The difference between He might have overheard you and He might not have overheard you (with internal negation) is that between "It is possible that he overheard you" and "It is possible that he did not overhear you".
By contrast, what is called external negation applies to the matrix clause. The difference between He could have overheard you and He couldn't have overheard you (with external negation) is that between "It is possible that he overheard you" and "It is not possible that he overheard you".
Not can be moved into the subordinate clause. He might have not overheard you has the same meaning as He might not have overheard you; but He could have not overheard you means "It is possible that he did not overhear you".
The two kinds of negation can be combined. He can't have not overheard you means "It is not possible that he did not overhear you".
Likewise, the difference between You mustn't apologize and You needn't apologize is that the former shows internal negation, inverting the necessity; the latter external negation, negating the necessity.
Whether negating a modal auxiliary verb brings negation of the subordinate or the matrix clause (internal or external negation respectively) thus depends on the particular verb, which in turn partly depends on the strength of the modality that the verb expresses, and there may be other determining factors as well. However: "Negative interrogatives, used as questions biased towards a positive answer, have external negation irrespective of the strength of the modality [. . .] A special case is in tags: We must stop soon, mustn't we?"
Usage of specific verbs
Can and could
The modal verb can expresses possibility in a dynamic, deontic, or epistemic sense, that is, in terms of innate ability, permissibility, or probability. For example:
- Dynamic
- Ability:You needn't struggle with your Tamil when talking to me: I can speak English ("I am capable of speaking English"; "I know how to speak English")
- Existential:Most siblings get along at least tolerably well, but there can be strong rivalry between them (such rivalry does sometimes occur)
- The reasonable/acceptable:You can be a few minutes late; nobody will mind
- The circumstantially possible:Petrol left for months in an unused car can wreck its fuel line (This is the result of a predictable chemical process that is not being prevented.)
- Deontic:Smoking is forbidden anywhere in this building, but you can smoke behind the bicycle shed ("You are permitted to smoke here")
- Epistemic:He did the "Ironman" in under seven hours? That can't be true. ("It is impossible for that to be true.")
The preterite form could is used as the past tense or remote conditional form of can in the above meanings (see § Preterite forms above). It is also used to express likelihood: We could be in trouble here. It is preferable to use could, may or might rather than can when expressing likelihood in a particular situation[citation needed] (as opposed to the general case, as in the "rivalry" example above, where can or may is used).
Both can and could can be used to make requests:Can/could you pass me the cheese? means "Please pass me the cheese" (where could is more polite). Either can be used with possibly: Can/could you possibly pass me the cheese? Requests with can't may sound impatient (Can't you be quiet?)
It is common to use can with verbs of perception such as see, hear, etc., as in I can see a tree.Aspectual distinctions can be made, such as I could see it (ongoing state) vs. I saw it (event).[citation needed]
Could have expresses counterfactual past ability or possibility: I could have told him if I had seen him; I could have told him yesterday (but I didn't).
Can have... is less common than may have....[citation needed]
Can may be negated by the addition of not /kən ˈnɒt/, analogously to the addition of not to could, may, will and so forth. It can also be negated by inflection; its commoner inflected form is can't /ˈkɑnt/, /ˈkɑnt/, or /kant/ (in RP, General American and General Australian respectively). However, it has an alternative inflected form, cannot /ˈkænɒt/. Can not and cannot thus differ in placement of the single stress. Can not is more formal than can't, and does not invert with its subject (Can't/*Cannot we leave now?).
Negated, could has the inflected form couldn't.
Negating can or could is external and negates the matrix clause, expressing inability, impermissibility or impossibility (I can't wear jeans). This differs from may or might used to express possibility: It can't be true does not mean It may not be true. Thus can't (or cannot) is often used to express disbelief even in possibility, as must expresses belief in the certainty. When the reference is to the past, have is used: He can't/cannot have done it means "It is not possible that he did it" (compare He must have done it).
With special stress, internal negation is possible: I can NOT wear a suit, if I wish means "I am not compelled to wear a suit if I don't want to".
May and might
The verb may expresses possibility in either an epistemic or deontic sense, that is, in terms of probability or permissibility. For example:
- The mouse may be dead means that it is possible that the mouse (perhaps audible until the day before) is now dead.
- Trevor may leave if he'd prefer to play with his friends means that Trevor is permitted to leave.
May can have future as well as present reference (He may arrive means that it is possible that he will arrive; I may go to the mall means that I am considering going to the mall).
The preterite form might is used as a synonym of may to express a possible circumstance (as can could – see above). It is sometimes said[according to whom?] that might and could express more doubt than may. For uses of might in conditional sentences, and as a past equivalent to may in such contexts as indirect speech, see § Preterite forms above.
May (or might) can also express concession of a minor point: He may be taller than me, but he's certainly not stronger could mean "While I'd agree that he is taller than me, that is unimportant, as he's certainly not stronger."
May can indicate permission for present or future actions, or be a polite directive: You may go now. Might used in this way is milder: You might go now if you feel like it. Similarly, May I use your phone? is a request for permission; Might I use your phone? would be more hesitant or polite.
A less common use of may is optative (to express a wish), as in May you live long and happy (see also English subjunctive).
May have indicates uncertainty about a past circumstance, whereas might have can either have that meaning or refer to possibilities that did not occur but could have (see also conditional sentences above).
- She may have eaten the cake. (The speaker does not know whether she did.)
- She might have eaten the cake. (The speaker either does not know whether she did, or knows that she did not eat cake but that her eating it would have been possible.)
May have is used for possibility, not permission (although the second sense of might have might sometimes imply permission).
The inflected form mayn't is obsolete. The inflected form mightn't mostly appears in the tags of tag questions (It might snow tonight, mightn't it?) and in other questions expressing doubt (Mightn't I come in if I took my boots off?).
The result of negating may or might depends on whether the interpretation is epistemic (about likelihood) or deontic (about permission). In epistemic senses, the negation is "internal", of the subordinate clause (There may not be a vote on it this week). In deontic senses, the negation is normally external (You may not go to the party unless you finish your homework), but with special stress, internal negation is possible: (I may NOT attend church, if I wish, meaning "I have permission not to attend church").
Shall and should
The verb shall is used in some varieties of English in place of will when the subject is first person (I shall, we shall).
With second- and third-person subjects, shall indicates a directive or prophecy: Cinderella, you shall go to the ball! It is often used in writing laws and specifications: Those convicted of violating this law shall be imprisoned for a term of not less than three years; The device shall be able to operate within a normal temperature range.
Shall is sometimes used in requests for advice or confirmation of a suggestion: Shall I read now?; What shall we wear?
Should is sometimes used as a first-person equivalent to would (in its conditional and "future-in-the-past" uses) in the same way that shall can replace will. Should is also used for a protasis with future reference: either with the preposition if (If you should meet her, please give her this) or with subject–auxiliary inversion (Should you meet her, please give her this).
Should is often used to describe an expected or recommended act or state. It can be used to give advice or to describe normative behavior, though without such strong obligatory force as must or have to. Thus, You should never lie describes a social or ethical norm. It can also express what is expected: This should work. In these uses it is equivalent to ought.
Both shall and should can be used with have (shall/should have (done)) in their role as first-person equivalents of will and would (thus to form future perfect or conditional perfect structures). Also, shall have may express an order with perfective aspect (You shall have finished your duties by nine o'clock). When should is used in that way, it usually expresses something that would have been expected at some time in the past but did not in fact happen (or is not known to have happened): I should have done that yesterday ("It would have been expedient, or expected of me, to do that yesterday").
The negative inflections are shan't and shouldn't.
Negating should negates the subordinate clause: the negation is internal (You shouldn't use botox). (To negate the meaning of I should, one may useI ought not to or I am not supposed to.)
As for any modal auxiliary, a negative interrogative (Shouldn't you check your credit card statement?) instead negates the matrix clause.
Will and would
- Will often expresses futurity (The next meeting will be held on Thursday). Since this is an expression of time rather than modality, constructions with will (or sometimes shall; see "Shall and should" above) are often called the future tense. For those speakers who for first-person subjects (I, we) use shall to express futurity, the use of will for these indicates particular resolve. (Future events are also sometimes described with the present tense (see Uses of English verb forms), or using the going to construction.)
- Will can express habitual aspect or dynamic modality; for example, He will make mistakes (in which will is usually stressed somewhat) may mean that he seems often to make them.
Will also has these uses as a modal:
- It can express strong probability with present time reference, as in That will be John at the door.
- It can be used to give an indirect order, as in You will do it right now.
Modal uses of the preterite form would include:
- Would is used in some conditional sentences.[vague]
- Expression of politeness, as in I would like to... (to politely state a preference) and Would you (be so kind as to) do this? (for "Please do this").
As a tense marker, would is used for
- Future of the past, as in I knew I would graduate two years later. Would is the past form of future will as described above under § Preterite forms. (It is sometimes replaced by should in the first person in the same way that will is replaced by shall.)
As an aspect marker, would is used for
- Expression of habitual aspect in the past, as in Back then, I would eat early and would walk to school.
Both will and would can be used with have (will have, would have) to form the future perfect and conditional perfect forms already referred to, or to express perfective aspect in their other meanings (e.g. there will have been an arrest order, expressing strong probability).
The negative inflections are won't and wouldn't. For contracted forms of will and would themselves, see "Weak forms", above.
Negating will or would is "internal" and negates the subordinate clause. (I won't be surprised if it rains means I will be unsurprised if it rains.) But as for any modal auxiliary, a negative interrogative (Won't/Wouldn't we submit them in person?) negates the matrix clause.
Would rather, would sooner, and would as soon
Would rather, would sooner, or would as soon can take as its complement either a bare infinitival clause (She would rather go herself) or a declarative content clause (She would rather (that) I went). They are PPIs: although I would rather not catch the virus (with negation of the clause that is subordinate to would rather) is idiomatic, *I wouldn't rather catch the virus (with negation of the matrix clause) is distinctly strange. Whether its reference is to past, present or future, the declarative content clause complement can use the preterite: I'd rather you hadn't told her that (past counterfactual); I'd rather you didn't tell her that (present/future); I'd rather you didn't tell her that when you meet her (future).
Must
Must differs from the central modal auxiliary verbs in lacking a preterite. It expresses obligation or necessity: You must use this form; We must try to escape. It can also express a conclusion reached by indirect evidence (e.g. Sue must be at home).
When used with have and a past participle, must has only an epistemic flavor: Sue must have left means that the speaker concludes that Sue has left. To express obligation or necessity in the past, had to or some other synonym must be used.
The negative inflection of must is mustn't. Negation of must is "internal", negating the subordinate clause: (You must not/mustn't drive after smoking a joint means that not driving is what you must do). But as for any modal auxiliary, a negative interrogative (Mustn't we hide the dope?) negates the matrix clause. To express the lack of requirement or obligation, the negative of have to or need (see below) can be used: You don't have to do this; You needn't do this.
Negated, must is not commonly used in an epistemic sense, where it is common to use can't (It can't be here; Sue can't have left) instead.
Mustn't can nonetheless be used as a simple negative of must in tag questions and other questions expressing doubt: We must do it, mustn't we? Mustn't he be in the operating room by this stage?
Must and needs can occur in sequence. Hendrik Poutsma writes that "The force of must, notably that of representing the subject under pressure of an overmastering desire [. . . ], is often emphasized by needs." Examples of the pair are:
- The control of the Firm needs must stay within the Family.
- It needs must be said that any observation made in this order shall not be taken as observations on merits
- thinking it through to its ultimate logical consequences must needs lead to insoluble contradictions
- the Constitution envisions, and by extension the country as a whole must needs have, a truly high-minded Supreme Court
Ought
Ought differs from the central modal auxiliary verbs both in taking as its complement a to-infinitival rather than a bare infinitival clause (compare He should go with He ought to go) and in lacking a preterite.
Ought is used with meanings similar to those of should, expressing expectation or requirement.
The reduced pronunciation of ought to (see "Weak forms" above) is sometimes spelt oughtta.
Ought can be used with have in the same way as should (plus intervening to): You ought to have done that earlier. Ought not to or oughtn't to can be substituted for shouldn't.
Had better has a similar meaning to should and ought for a deontic meaning (expressing recommended or expedient behavior (You ought to / should / had better arrive on time), but not (other perhaps than jokingly) for an epistemic meaning (The Sun ought to / should / ?had better come out soon).
Negating ought is "internal" and negates the subordinate clause (I ought not to have a third glassful means that what I ought to do is decline the glassful). But as for any modal auxiliary, a negative interrogative (Oughtn't we to offer cola as well as beer?) negates the matrix clause.
The use of ought as a lexical verb as in They didn't ought to go is generally thought of as restricted to nonstandard dialects but has been described as also sometimes found in informal standard usage. "Lexical ought with the dummy operator do has been condemned in British usage handbooks. . . . What this censure suggests is that lexical ought with periphrastic do is a well-established usage in colloquial [British English]."
Data from a corpus of American and British spoken and written English of the 1980s and 1990s show that ought not to, oughtn't to (both modal auxiliary) and didn't ought to are rare in both American and British English, whether written or spoken. I don't think you ought to and similar are commonly used instead. In interrogatives, ought does not appear in American conversation or fiction or in British conversation. In British fiction, the modal auxiliary is used (Ought we to . . . ?), not lexical ought with do-support.
Need
As a modal auxiliary verb, need is a negative polarity item, appearing in negative contexts and other contexts that do not affirm. Thus:
- We need not remain silent.
- Need we remain silent?
- *We need remain silent.
Like must, modal need has no preterite form.
Although as a modal auxiliary verb need takes a bare infinitival clause complement (He needn't overhaul it), lexical verb need can take either an object complement (He needs my help) or a to-infinitival clause complement (He needs to overhaul it), optionally with a subject (He needs me to overhaul it).
Negation of need is external, negating the matrix clause. You needn't apply again does not say that there is a need not to apply, merely that there is no need to apply. So although the verb must can usually be substituted for the modal verb need, mustn't usually cannot be substituted for needn't. (Exceptionally, the pair are synonymous in polar interrogatives: Needn't/mustn't we pay now?)
Modal need can also be used with have: Need I have done that? It is most commonly used here in the negative, meaning that an action was (from the present perspective) not in fact necessary: You needn't have left that tip.
Data from a corpus of American and British spoken and written English of the 1980s and 1990s show that for negative constructions involving need, modal auxiliary need is more common in written English (both American and British, but is less common than lexical need in British English conversation and unused in American English conversation.163 In both American and British English, interrogative constructions that require subject–auxiliary inversion show do-support of lexical need much more commonly than inversion of auxiliary need; moreover, many of what instances there are of auxiliary need are of fixed formulas (Need I say more?, etc).
For "needs must" (and "must needs") see under must.
Dare
As a modal auxiliary verb, dare is another negative negative polarity item, appearing in negative contexts and other contexts that do not affirm.
Dare is now much more common as a lexical verb. Lexical verb dare takes a to-infinitival clause as its complement (I didn't dare to answer her), and this may have a subject (He dared me to dive from a higher board); modal dare, a bare infinitival clause complement.
Negation of dare is external: what is negated is the matrix clause. (She dare not attempt it means "She doesn't dare to attempt it".)
Examples of the use of modal auxiliary dare, followed by equivalents using lexical dare where appropriate:
- If he dare try it, he may succeed. ("If he dares to try it, he may succeed.")
- If he dared try it, he might succeed. ("If he dared to try it, he might succeed.")
- Dare he do it? ("Does he dare to do it?")
- Dared he do it? ("Did he dare to do it?")
- I daren't (or dare not) try. ("I don't dare to try.")
- I dared not try. ("I didn't dare to try.")
- How dare you! (formulaic expression of outrage)
- I dare say (or daresay) it's true. (Another formulaic expression, here exceptionally in an affirmative context, unexpected for an NPI)
However, its affirmative context causes *He dared speak up to be ungrammatical.
Although seemingly obsolete in the 21st century, daredn't was in use in the early 20th:
- "I daredn't hurry," said Mr Colclough, setting us down at the station. "I was afraid of a skid."
- One's so safe with such a son to con her / Through all the noises and through all the press, / Boys daredn't squirt tormenters on her dress.[83]
Dared has supplanted an earlier preterite form, durst. Examples:
- The former [. . .] retired with cattle and other booty to their mountains, whither they knew well the Lowlanders durst not follow them.[85]
- Other debts I durst not face.[86]
- dizzy church-crowned central peak that time durst not touch[87]
Durst had a negative inflected form, durstn't. Examples:
- I'm under authority, you know, and durstn't overstep[88]
- the boat, where I durstn't kick for fear of poking my feet through the bottom[89]
- I durstn't go home to tell Mother Pring[90]
Lexical verb dare is close to an NPI: She dared to speak up is much less likely than She didn't dare to speak up. And the lexical–modal distinction is blurred: "lexical dare commonly occurs in non-affirmative contexts without to": She wouldn't dare ask her father; and it also can be stranded, as in She ought to have asked for a raise, but she didn't dare.
Data from a corpus of American and British spoken and written English of the 1980s and 1990s show that dare (modal or lexical) is infrequent and "is found chiefly in fiction and [British English] conversation". In negative constructions in American fiction, lexical dare is more common. In Britain, modal auxiliary dare is. Further, negation of preterite dared is rare. In both American and British English, interrogative constructions that require subject–auxiliary inversion show inversion of auxiliary dare much more commonly than do-support of lexical dare; however, many of the instances here of auxiliary dare are of fixed formulas (How dare you . . . ?, etc).
Used
Used /just/ is far more commonly encountered as a lexical than as an auxiliary verb, particularly for younger or American speakers. The plain form use (sometimes spelt ⟨used⟩) of the lexical verb is seen in Did you use to play tennis?). Although rare, its preterite perfect had used is attested. The first of The Cambridge Grammar of the English Language's five criteria for modal auxiliary verbs is irrelevant to auxiliary verb used, which fails the last three. The auxiliary verb "is also semantically quite distinct from the modal auxiliaries: the meaning it expresses is aspectual, not modal."The Cambridge Grammar does not class auxiliary used as a modal auxiliary verb.
For more about use, see English auxiliary verbs.
Modal idioms with have
The verb had in the expression had better lacks any untensed form (*Tomorrow you will have better concentrate; *I've had better work hard since I started; *We're having better concentrate) and hence is sometimes classed as a modal idiom, a semi-modal, or an emerging or quasi-modal verb.
Negating had better, whether by had better not or by hadn't better, normally negates the subordinate clause: it is internal (You'd better not stick around). However, as for any modal auxiliary, a negative interrogative (Hadn't we better scarper before the police come?) negates the matrix clause.
Had best and had rather similarly lack any untensed form. Had best is much less common than had better. Since had rather and would rather are both likely to be realized as 'd rather, it is rarely easy to decide which of the pair is being used.
Hendrik Poutsma adds:
I had as lief (or lieve), although now antiquated and mostly replaced by I had as soon, has never fallen completely into disuse. . . . The shortening of had to 'd has given rise to would being sometimes substituted for it.
Frequency of use
During the second half of the 20th century, the frequencies of use of both the modal auxiliary verbs and of alternatives to them showed considerable change. A comparison of the frequencies in the British corpora LOB and FLOB (with material from 1961 and 1991 respectively), and of those in the American corpora Brown and Frown (1961 and 1992 material respectively) shows:
Modals | Quasi-modals | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
BrE | AmE | BrE | AmE | |||
can | (+2.2%) | (–1.5%) | be going to | (–1.2%) | +51.6% | |
could | (+2.4%) | –6.8% | be to | –17.2% | –40.1% | |
may | –17.4% | –32.4% | had better | (–26.0%) | (–17.1%) | |
might | –15.1% | (–4.5%) | have got to | (–34.1%) | (+15.6%) | |
must | –29.0% | –34.4% | have to | (+9.0%) | (+1.1%) | |
need | –40.2% | (–12.5%) | need to | +249.1% | +123.2% | |
ought to | –44.2% | (–30.0%) | be supposed to | +113.6% | (+6.3%) | |
shall | –43.7% | –43.8% | want to | +18.5% | +70.9% | |
should | –11.8% | –13.5% | ||||
will | (–2.7%) | –11.1% | ||||
would | –11.0% | –6.1% |
(Percentage changes shown in parentheses come with χ2 values of greater than 0.05; they are of less statistical significance.)
A study of modal auxiliary verbs and quasi-modals in American, British and Australian examples (given equal weight) of a variety of genres of written and spoken English in the 1990s found that the totals were:
Modals | Quasi-modals | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
can | 7663 | be able to | 889 | |
could | 3557 | be about to | 124 | |
may | 2261 | be bound to | 27 | |
might | 1499 | be going to | 2721 | |
must | 1367 | be supposed to | 171 | |
need | 56 | be to | 371 | |
ought to | 126 | had better | 89 | |
shall | 343 | have got to | 705 | |
should | 2432 | have to | 2827 | |
will | 8505 | need to | 716 | |
would | 7775 | want to | 1897 | |
Total | 35584 | Total | 10537 |
Commenting on a different but similar set of figures, Longman Grammar of Spoken and Written English observes of ought, need, dare, and use /jus/:
In view of the considerable attention given to these marginal auxiliaries in grammatical descriptions of English and English language teaching materials, it is worth noting how rare they are, particularly in negative and interrogative auxiliary constructions.
Deduction
In English, modal verbs as must, have, got and could/can are used to express deduction and contention. The modal verbs state how sure the speaker is about something.
- You're shivering – you must be cold.
- Someone must have taken the key: it is not here.
- I didn't order ten books. This has to be a mistake.
- These aren't mine – they've got to be yours.
- It can't be a burglar. All the doors and windows are locked.
Modals at the head of chains
The verb governed by the modal may be another auxiliary (necessarily one that can appear in plain form—this includes be and have, but not another modal, except in the non-standard cases described below under § Double modals). Hence, a modal may introduce a chain of verb forms in which the other auxiliaries express properties such as aspect and voice, as in He must have been given a new job. If infinitival to is regarded as an auxiliary verb, then longer chains are possible, as in He must have been encouraged to try to serve tea.
Double modals
In Standard English, since a modal auxiliary verb is followed by a verb in its plain form (which modals lack), it cannot be followed by a second modal auxiliary verb. Might have is grammatical (have is here the plain form of a non-modal verb), but *might must is not.
However, what appear to be sequences of modal auxiliary verbs occur. Might could, must can, might oughta, might would, must could, could oughta, might should, may can, should oughta, might can, may could, would oughta, might will, may will, may should are some of the 76 combinations attested in Southern American English. Those with might as the first modal are easily the most common, and might could is the most common of them all. Longer sequences such as might should oughta are also attested. In Britain, by contrast, the most common is would might although commonness is relative: double modal auxiliary verbs "occur only rarely in spontaneous speech, even in varieties in which they are known to be used".
The syntactic status of sequences such as might could and would might is unclear. One possibility is that might has been reanalysed by the speaker as an English adverbs and thus be functioning as an adjunct.
Two rules from different grammatical models supposedly disallow the construction. Phrase structure grammar sees the surface clause as allowing only one modal verb, and main verb analysis dictates that modal verbs occur in finite forms.
Comparison with other Germanic languages
Many English modals have cognates in other Germanic languages, if often with different meanings. Unlike the English modals, however, such verbs are not generally defective:
- In German: mögen, müssen, können, sollen, wollen; cognates of may, must, can, shall, and will. Although German shares five modal verbs with English, their meanings are often quite different. Mögen does not mean "be allowed" but "may" as epistemic modal and "like" as a normal verb followed by a noun phrase. It can be followed by an infinitive with the meaning of "have a desire (to do something)". Wollen means "will" only in the sense of "want (to do something)" and is not used for future reference, for which werden is used instead. Müssen, können, and sollen are used similarly to English "must", "can", and "shall". The negation of müssen is a literal one in German, not an inverse one as in English": German ich muss ("I must") means "I need (to do something)", and ich muss nicht (literally "I must not") accordingly means "I don't need (to do something)". In English, "have (to do something)" behaves the same way, whereas English "must" expresses an interdiction when negated. brauchen (need) is sometimes used like a modal verb, especially negated (Er braucht nicht kommen. "He need not come.").
- In Dutch: mogen, moeten, kunnen, zullen, willen; cognates of may, must, can, shall, and will.
- In Danish: måtte, kunne, ville, skulle, cognates of may/must, can, will, shall. They generally have the same meanings as in English, with the exception of ville, which usually means "want (to do something)" (but can also mean "will").
- In Swedish: må (past tense: måtte), måsta, kunna, vilja, ska(ll), cognates of may/might, must, can, will, shall. Their meanings generally correspond to those in English with the exception of vilja, which means "want (to do something)".
Since modal verbs in other Germanic languages are not defective, the problem of double modals (see above) does not arise: the second modal verb in such a construction simply takes the infinitive form like for any verb in the same position. Compare the following translations of English "I want to be able to dance", all of which translate literally as "I want can dance" (except the German, as "I want dance can"):
See also
- Tense–aspect–mood § Invariant auxiliaries
Notes
- Although there are non-auxiliary modal verbs in English, such as "require" and "oblige", the aim of brevity causes this article often to use "modal" or "modal verb" to mean "modal auxiliary verb".
- The plain form of a verb is exemplified by beware and the form be of the verb be: You should beware of the dog; You should be careful. It is distinguished from the plain present form of the verb, as exemplified by am, are, and were. For any verb other than be that is not defective, the plain and plain present forms are identical in pronunciation and spelling.
- More precisely: Does there exist a lexical verb with the same spelling and pronunciation that is synonymous or could be said to have an auxiliary (or copular) function? (Ignored here is any lexical verb – will meaning "exert one's will in an attempt to compel", can meaning "insert into cans", etc – that is unlikely to be mistaken for the auxiliary verb.)
- "[T]here is evidence that for some speakers [of Standard English] may and might have diverged to the extent that they are no longer inflectional forms of a single lexeme, but belong to distinct lexemes, may and might, each of which – like must – lacks a preterite. . . ."
- An NPI, rare for speakers of Standard American English.
- Pronounced /just/ (rhyming with "roost"). Auxiliary verb form used should be distinguished from the homonymous adjective used, as in I've got (very) used to it. (The homographic verb form used /juzd/, rhyming with "refused", is lexical only.) "For many speakers [of Standard English], especially younger ones", use /jus/ is exclusively a lexical verb.
- This article uses asterisks ⟨*⟩ to indicate ungrammatical expressions. As an example, "She can/*cans try" means that although She can try is grammatical, *She cans try is not.
References
- Huddleston & Pullum (2002), p. 104.
- Aarts (2011), pp. 83–85.
- Quirk et al. (1985), [page needed].
- Palmer (1988), pp. 242–248.
- Huddleston & Pullum (2002), p. 109.
- Zandvoort (1975), p. 85.
- Huddleston & Pullum (2002), p. 115.
- Huddleston & Pullum (2002), pp. 94–107.
- Huddleston & Pullum (2002), p. 91.
- Huddleston & Pullum (2002), pp. 74–75.
- Huddleston & Pullum (2002), pp. 109, 115.
- Huddleston & Pullum (2002), pp. 109, 113.
- Huddleston & Pullum (2002), pp. 108–110.
- Palmer (2001), p. 33.
- Palmer (1965), [page needed].
- Levine (2012), pp. 187–204.
- Quirk et al. (1985), pp. 135–136.
- Palmer (1988), p. 26.
- Warner (1993), p. 11.
- Huddleston & Pullum (2002), pp. 108–109.
- Aarts (2011), pp. 280–298.
- Huddleston & Pullum (2002), p. 108.
- Collins (2009), p. 14.
- Quirk et al. (1985), pp. 138–140.
- Huddleston & Pullum (2002), pp. 109–111.
- Aarts (2011), pp. 298–301.
- Quirk et al. (1985), pp. 138, 140.
- Palmer (1988), p. 170.
- Aarts (2011), pp. 273–274.
- Palmer (1988), pp. 170–171.
- Quirk et al. (1985), pp. 141–143.
- Aarts (2011), pp. 303–304.
- Huddleston & Pullum (2002), pp. 108, 113.
- Palmer (1988), pp. 128–131, 141–143.
- Warner (1993), p. 46.
- Huddleston & Pullum (2002), pp. 111–114.
- Aarts (2011), pp. 301–302, 304–305.
- Palmer (1988), p. 106.
- Quirk et al. (1985), pp. 143–146.
- OED (1989).
- Huddleston & Pullum (2002), p. 1613.
- Huddleston & Pullum (2002), p. 1615.
- Huddleston & Pullum (2002), p. 175.
- Huddleston & Pullum (2002), pp. 175, 1216.
- Huddleston & Pullum (2002), p. 176.
- Huddleston & Pullum (2002), p. 205.
- Huddleston & Pullum (2002), p. 185.
- Huddleston & Pullum (2002), p. 184.
- Huddleston & Pullum (2002), p. 183.
- Huddleston & Pullum (2002), p. 181.
- Huddleston & Pullum (2002), p. 865.
- Huddleston & Pullum (2002), p. 940.
- Huddleston & Pullum (2002), pp. 768, 770.
- Huddleston & Pullum (2002), p. 1611.
- Huddleston & Pullum (2002), pp. 180, 184, 204, 804–805.
- Huddleston & Pullum (2002), pp. 856, 944.
- Huddleston & Pullum (2002), pp. 180, 182, 184, 204.
- Koltai (2013).
- Huddleston & Pullum (2002), pp. 187, 204.
- Fleischman (1982), pp. 86–97.
- Comrie (1985), pp. 21, 47–48.
- Ultralingua (n.d.).
- StudySpanish (n.d.).
- Huddleston & Pullum (2002), pp. 193, 205.
- Huddleston & Pullum (2002), p. 829.
- Huddleston & Pullum (2002), pp. 1003–1004.
- Huddleston & Pullum (2002), pp. 180, 182, 204, 205.
- Poutsma (1929), p. 61.
- Kapoor (2013).
- Daily Excelsior (2018).
- von Mises (2020).
- Adler (2018).
- Huddleston & Pullum (2002), pp. 187, 205.
- Quirk et al. (1985), p. 140.
- Greenbaum (1996), p. 155.
- Lee & Collins (2004), p. 502.
- Biber et al. (1999), p. 165.
- Biber et al. (1999), p. 218.
- Huddleston & Pullum (2002), p. 180.
- Biber et al. (1999), p. 217.
- Huddleston & Pullum (2002), p. 196.
- Bennett (1971), p. 186.
- Masefield (1912), [page needed].
- Huddleston & Pullum (2002), p. 110n.
- Worsaae (1852), [page needed].
- Yonge (1875), [page needed].
- Lovecraft (2022), [page needed].
- Ballantyne (2007), [page needed].
- Fenn (2007), [page needed].
- Blackmore (2021), [page needed].
- Huddleston & Pullum (2002), pp. 110–111.
- Huddleston & Pullum (2002), p. 110.
- Biber et al. (1999), p. 163.
- Biber et al. (1999), p. 164.
- Biber et al. (1999), pp. 217–218.
- Huddleston & Pullum (2002), p. 92.
- Collins (2009), p. 16.
- Leech et al. (2009), pp. 105–106.
- Krug (2009), p. 332.
- Huddleston & Pullum (2002), pp. 196, 205.
- Van der Auwera, Noël & Van linden (2013), p. 6.
- Poutsma (1929), p. 158.
- Collins (2009), p. 7.
- Mair & Leech (2006), pp. 327–328.
- Collins (2009), p. 5.
- Biber et al. (1999), p. 219.
- Learn English (n.d.).
- Murphy (2013).
- Yule (2006), p. 40.
- Morin & Grieve (2024), pp. 11–13.
- Hasty (2012), pp. 1717–1718.
- Coats (2023), pp. 11, 17.
- Coats (2023), p. 2.
- Lebedeva & Orlova (2019), pp. 71–84.
- Di Paolo (1989), p. 195.
Works cited
- Aarts, Bas (2011). Oxford Modern English Grammar. Oxford: Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-953319-0.
- Adler, Bruce K. (15 October 2018). "How to depoliticize the US Supreme Court". Hippo Reads. Retrieved 19 December 2023.
- Ballantyne, R. M. (2007) [1876]. Under the Waves: Diving in Deep Waters – via Project Gutenberg.
- Bennett, Arnold (1971) [1907]. "The Death of Simon Fuge". The Grim Smile of the Five Towns. Harmondsworth, Middlesex: Penguin. ISBN 978-0-14-000519-6 – via Internet Archive.
- Biber, Douglas; Johansson, Stig; Leech, Geoffrey; Conrad, Susan; Finegan, Edward (1999). Longman Grammar of Spoken and Written English. Harlow, Essex: Pearson Education. ISBN 978-0-582-23725-4 – via Internet Archive.
- Blackmore, R. D. (2021) [1895]. Slain by the Doones – via Project Gutenberg.
- Coats, Steven (December 2023). "Double modals in contemporary British and Irish speech" (PDF). English Language and Linguistics. 27 (4): 693–718. doi:10.1017/S1360674323000126.
- Collins, Peter (2009). Modals and Quasi-modals in English. Language and Computers: Studies in Practical Linguistics 67. Amsterdam: Rodopi. ISBN 978-90-420-2532-5.
- Comrie, Bernard (1985). Tense. Cambridge Textbooks in Linguistics. Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0-521-28138-6.
- "HC rejects quashing of FIR in multi-crore FCI scam". Daily Excelsior. 3 August 2018. Retrieved 19 December 2023.
- Di Paolo, Marianna (1989). "Double modals as single lexical items". American Speech. 64 (3): 195–224. doi:10.2307/455589. JSTOR 455589.
- Fenn, George Manville (2007) [1891]. To the West – via Project Gutenberg.
- Fleischman, Suzanne (1982). The Future in Thought and Language: Diachronic Evidence from Romance. Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0-521-24389-6.
- Greenbaum, Sidney (1996). The Oxford English Grammar. Oxford: Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-861250-6.
- Hasty, J. Daniel (2012). "We might should oughta take a second look at this: A syntactic re-analysis of double modals in Southern United States English". Lingua. 122 (14): 1716–1738. doi:10.1016/j.lingua.2012.09.005.
- Huddleston, Rodney; Pullum, Geoffrey K. (2002). The Cambridge Grammar of the English Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0-521-43146-0.
- Kapoor, Virendra (26 January 2013). "Jaipur Shivir had one-point agenda". The Sunday Guardian. Retrieved 19 December 2023.
- Koltai, Anastasia (February 21, 2013). "English Grammar: Usage of Shall vs Should with Examples". My English Teacher.
- Krug, Manfred (2009). "Modality and the history of English adhortatives". In Salkie, Raphael; Busuttil, Pierre; van der Auwera, Johan (eds.). Modality in English: Theory and Description. Topics in English Linguistics 58. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. pp. 315–347. ISBN 978-3-11-019634-4.
- "Modals – deduction (present)". Learn English. British Council. n.d. Archived from the original on 2014-12-15.
- Lebedeva, Irina S.; Orlova, Svetlana N. (2019). "Semantics and pragmatics of the double modal 'might could'". Training, Language and Culture. 3 (2): 71–84. doi:10.29366/2019tlc.3.2.5 – via Cyberleninka.
- Lee, Jackie F. K.; Collins, Peter (2004). "On the usage of have, dare, need, ought and used to in Australian English and Hong Kong English". World Englishes. 23 (4): 501–513. doi:10.1111/j.0083-2919.2004.00374.x.
- Leech, Geoffrey; Hundt, Marianne; Mair, Christian; Smith, Nicholas (2009). Change in Contemporary English: A Grammatical Study. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0-521-86722-1.
- Levine, Robert D. (2012). "Auxiliaries: To's company". Journal of Linguistics. 48 (1): 187–203. doi:10.1017/S002222671100034X. ISSN 0022-2267.
- Lovecraft, H. P. (2022) [1924]. The Festival – via Project Gutenberg.
- von Mises, Ludwig (7 July 2020) [1944]. "Mises on the Velocity of Circulation". Mises Wire. Retrieved 19 December 2023.
- Mair, Christian; Leech, Geoffrey N. (2006). "Current changes in English syntax". In Aarts, Basil; McMahon, April (eds.). Handbook of English Linguistics. Oxford: Blackwell. pp. 318–342. ISBN 978-1-4051-1382-3.
- Masefield, John (1912). The Widow in the Bye Street. London: Sidgwick & Jackson. OCLC 330395 – via Gutenberg Project.
- Morin, Cameron; Grieve, Jack (2024). "The semantics, sociolinguistics, and origins of double modals in American English: New insights from social media". PLOS ONE. 19 (1): e0295799. Bibcode:2024PLoSO..1995799M. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0295799. PMC 10807846. PMID 38265988.
- Murphy, Chris (18 June 2013). "Modals Deduction Past". EC English Language Centres.
- "need". Oxford English Dictionary (Second ed.). 1989.
- Palmer, Frank Robert (2001). Mood and Modality. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0-521-80479-0.
- Palmer, F. R. (1988). The English Verb (2nd ed.). London: Longman. ISBN 978-0-582-01470-1.
- Palmer, Frank Robert (1965). A Linguistic Study of the English Verb. London: Longmans.
- Poutsma, H. (1929). A Grammar of Late Modern English (PDF). Vol. 1 (2nd ed.). Groningen: P. Noordhoff. p. 158. OCLC 697774306 – via Internet Archive.
- Quirk, Randolph; Greenbaum, Sidney; Leech, Geoffrey; Svartvik, Jan (1985). A Comprehensive Grammar of the English Language. London: Longman. ISBN 978-0-582-51734-9.
- "The Conditional Tense". Study Spanish. n.d.
- "Conditional tense". UltraLingua Online Dictionary & Grammar. n.d. Archived from the original on 2009-10-11.
- Van der Auwera, Johan; Noël, Dirk; Van linden, An (2013). "Had better, 'd better and better: Diachronic and transatlantic variation" (PDF). In Marín-Arrese, Juana I.; Carretero, Marta; Arús, Jorge; Van der Auwera, Johan (eds.). English Modality: Core, Periphery and Evidentiality. Topics in English Linguistics 81. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton. pp. 119–154. ISBN 978-3-11-028632-8. Retrieved 29 November 2023 – via University of Liège.
- Warner, Anthony R. (1993). English Auxiliaries: Structure and History. Cambridge Studies in Linguistics 66. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0-521-30284-5.
- Worsaae, J. J. A. (1852). An Account of the Danes and Norwegians in England, Scotland, and Ireland. London: John Murray – via Project Gutenberg.
- Yonge, Charlotte M. (1875). The Pillars of the House; or, Under Wode, under Rode. Vol. 2. London: Macmillan – via Project Gutenberg.
- Yule, George (2006). Oxford Practice Grammar (Advanced). Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-432754-1.
- Zandvoort, R. W. (1975). A Handbook of English Grammar (7th ed.). London: Longman. ISBN 978-0-582-55339-2.
Look up Appendix English modal verbs in Wiktionary the free dictionary The English modal auxiliary verbs are a subset of the English auxiliary verbs used mostly to express modality properties such as possibility and obligation They can most easily be distinguished from other verbs by their defectiveness they do not have participles or plain forms and by their lack of the ending e s for the third person singular 3 The central English modal auxiliary verbs are can with could may with might shall with should will with would and must A few other verbs are usually also classed as modals ought and in certain uses dare and need Use jus rhyming with loose is included as well Other expressions notably had better share some of their characteristics Modal auxiliary verbs distinguished grammaticallyA list of what tend to be regarded as modal auxiliary verbs in Modern English along with their inflected forms is shown in the following table Contractions are shown only if their orthography is distinctive There are also unstressed versions that are typically although not necessarily written in the standard way Where there is a blank the modal auxiliary verb lacks this form A corresponding lexical verb may have the form For example although the lexical verb need has a preterite form the modal auxiliary verb need does not English modal auxiliary verb paradigm Citation form Plain present Preterite Confusible lexical homonym Neutral Contr Negative Neutral Contr Negativewill will ll won t would d wouldn t nonemay may might mightn t nonecan can can t cannot could couldn t noneshall shall ll shan t should shouldn t nonemust must mustn t noneought ought oughtn t marginal art in some varieties citation needed need need needn t existsdare dare daren t dared existshad better had better d better better hadn t better noneused used usedn t existsCriteria for modal auxiliary verbs Descriptive grammars of English differ slightly on the criteria they set for modal auxiliary verbs According to The Cambridge Grammar of the English Language the criteria are as follows Auxiliary verbs Modal auxiliary verbs are a subset of auxiliary verbs and thus meet the criteria for these For lists of those criteria see the article English auxiliary verbs but among them are that the verbs i can invert with their subjects notably in questions Must I go ii can be negated with not I must not go Must I not go and iii have negative inflected forms won t wouldn t No untensed forms To illustrate untensed forms those of the irregular lexical verb take and the non modal auxiliary verb be are the plain take and be as in Take it I didn t take it and Don t be silly the gerund participles taking and being and the past participles taken and been Modal auxiliary verbs lack untensed forms Attempting to use them brings ungrammatical results I will can drive if I take ten lessons Canning drive would be helpful I have could canned drive since I was 18 Compare the grammaticality of non modal auxiliary verb be in I will be able to drive being able to drive and I have been able to drive No subject verb agreement This refers to agreement of a verb in present tense with its third person singular subject She can cans try Compare lexical verb try in She tries try and non modal auxiliary verb do in She does do try Had better and as an auxiliary verb used lack present tense forms Other than in the present tense even lexical verbs lack subject agreement and so this test is inapplicable to either had better or used Only a bare infinitival clause as complement Whereas the lexical verb seem takes a to infinitival clause It seemed to happen and the non modal auxiliary verb have takes a past participial clause complement It has happened a modal auxiliary verb can in principle take only a bare infinitival clause a subordinate clause with the plain form of the verb without to as its complement It can be a surprise It can to be a surprise It can being a surprise If they are modal auxiliary verbs then ought and used are exceptions to this although ought is increasingly used with a bare infinitival clause complement Bare infinitival clause complements are not unique to modal auxiliary verbs Do is a non modal auxiliary verb that takes one Did you move the piano help is a lexical verb that can do so I helped move the piano Ability to occur in remote apodosis An apodosis is the then half of a conditional statement The if half is the protasis Remote here means thought by the speaker to be unlikely or known by the speaker to be untrue If I were an elephant I would eat more apples Compare lexical verb eat in If I were an elephant I ate more apples and non modal auxiliary verb be in If I were an elephant I was able to eat more apples Must satisfies this only for a minority of speakers and it is questionable whether had better does so The Cambridge Grammar comments on may that here there is evidence that for some speakers may and might have diverged to the extent that they are no longer inflectional forms of a single lexeme but belong to distinct lexemes may and might each of which like must lacks a preterite Used does not satisfy this Preterite usable in the main clause for modal remoteness I could drive there I suppose If similarly intended as a doubtful or incredulous contemplation of an option for the future attempts at this with a lexical or non modal auxiliary verb are ungrammatical I drove there I suppose I was going to drive there I suppose Other than when used for backshift should has diverged in meaning so far from shall be usable here only with difficulty As they lack preterite forms must ought and need cannot be used in this way and so that criterion does not apply to them And used describes the past not the present or future Comments The following verbs shown in present preterite pairs satisfy or come close to satisfying all of the above criteria and can be classed as the central modal verbs of English can with could will with would may with might although the lack in today s Standard English of a negative present inflection mayn t means that it fails one of the criteria for auxiliary verbs shall with should although the semantic divergence of shall and should means that its success with one criterion is debatable must although its lack of a preterite see its etymology below means that it neither passes nor fails one of the criteria Even for lexical verbs preterite forms have uses besides referring to the past but for modal auxiliary verbs such uses are particularly important Could you pass me the sauce Without my phone I might easily be lost You should work harder I would avoid that street Ought dare need and used satisfy some of the criteria above and are more ought dare need or less used often categorized as modal verbs 15 Had better is sometimes called a modal idiom Other English auxiliary verbs appear in a variety of different forms and are not regarded as modal verbs be used as an auxiliary verb in passive voice continuous aspect and indeed in virtually all of its uses even as a copula have used as an auxiliary verb in perfect aspect constructions and the idiom have got to it is also used in have to which has modal meaning but here as when denoting possession very broadly understood have only rarely follows auxiliary verb syntax do see do support to of to infinitival clauses if to is a defective verb not a subordinator Lists of modal auxiliary verbs Five recent scholarly descriptions of verbs disagree among themselves on the extension of modal auxiliary verb on which verbs are modal auxiliary verbs They agree that can with could may with might must shall with should and will with would are or are among the central modal auxiliaries A Comprehensive Grammar of the English Language 1985 secondary or modal auxiliaries F R Palmer 1988 modal auxiliaries Anthony R Warner 1993 central members of the modal auxiliary class The Cambridge Grammar of the English Language 2002 or core modal verbs Bas Aarts 2011 Among these five verbs The Cambridge Grammar selects the pair can and will with could and would as the most straightforward of the modal auxiliaries Peter Collins agrees All five accord ought need and dare a less clear or merely a marginal membership A Comprehensive Grammar and Warner do likewise for use the other three deny that it is a modal auxiliary verb For that reason it is discussed primarily not in this article but in English auxiliary verbs As for would in would rather would sooner and would as soon and have in had better had best and had rather only The Cambridge Grammar notes all six but each of the other four descriptions of auxiliary verbs notes three or more Of the three to six idioms that each discussion notes there is no variation in the status that it accords to them Warner calls the three that he notes would rather had better had rather modal auxiliaries Palmer says that the same three are not modal auxiliaries Both A Comprehensive Grammar and Aarts use the term modal idiom for a choice of five The Cambridge Grammar sees modal characteristics in all six uses of these two auxiliary verbs A Comprehensive Grammar calls both have got I ve got to go now and be to You are to hand over the cash modal idioms None of the other descriptions agrees Palmer calls be bound able going willing to and have got to semi modals A Comprehensive Grammar calls be able about apt wbr bound due going wbr likely meant obliged wbr supposed willing to and have to semi auxiliaries He adds The boundaries of this category are not clear EtymologyThe modals can and could are from Old English can n and cuth which were respectively the present and preterite forms of the verb cunnan be able The silent l in the spelling of could results from analogy with would and should Similarly may and might are from Old English maeg and meahte respectively the present and preterite forms of magan may to be able shall and should are from sceal and sceolde respectively the present and preterite forms of sculan owe be obliged and will and would are from wille and wolde respectively the present and preterite forms of willan wish want The aforementioned Old English verbs cunnan magan sculan and willan followed the preterite present paradigm or in the case of willan a similar but irregular paradigm which explains the absence of the ending s in the third person present forms can may shall and will The original Old English forms given above were first and third person singular forms their descendant forms have become generalized to all persons and numbers The verb must comes from Old English moste part of the verb motan be able obliged to do something This was another preterite present verb of which moste was in fact the preterite the present form mot gave rise to mote which was used as a modal verb in Early Modern English but must has now lost its past connotations and has replaced mote Similarly ought was originally a past form it derives from ahte preterite of agan own another Old English preterite present verb whose present tense form ah has also given the modern regular verb owe and ought was formerly used as a preterite form of owe The verb dare also originates from a preterite present verb durran dare specifically its present tense dear r although in its non modal uses in Modern English it is conjugated regularly However need comes from the regular Old English verb neodian meaning be necessary the alternative third person form need in place of needs which has become the norm in modal uses became common in the 16th century Preterite formsThe preterite forms given above could might should and would corresponding to can may shall and will respectively do not always simply modify the meaning of the modal to give it past reference The only one regularly used as an ordinary past tense is could when referring to ability I could swim may serve as a past form of I can swim All the preterites are used as past equivalents for the corresponding present modals in indirect speech and similar clauses requiring the rules of sequence of tenses to be applied For example if it were said in 1960 that People think that we will all be driving hovercars by the year 2000 it might now be reported that In 1960 people thought we would all be driving hovercars by the year 2000 This future in the past also known as the past prospective use of would can also occur in a main clause I moved to Green Gables in 1930 I would live there for the next ten years In many cases in order to give modals past reference they are used together with the auxiliary have and a past participle as in I should have asked her You may have seen me Sometimes these expressions are limited in meaning for example must have can refer only to certainty whereas past obligation is expressed by an alternative phrase such as had to see Replacements for defective forms below Conditional sentences The preterite forms of modals are used in the apodosis then clause of counterfactual conditional sentences The modal would or should as a first person alternative is used to produce the conditional construction typically used in clauses of this type If you loved me you would support me It can be replaced by could meaning would be able to do something and might meaning would possibly as appropriate When the clause has past reference the construction with the modal plus have see above is used If they had wanted to do it they would could might have done it by now The would have done construction is called the conditional perfect The protasis if clause of such a sentence typically contains the preterite form of a verb or the past perfect construction for past reference without any modal The modal could may be used here in its role of the preterite form of can if I could speak French However all the modal preterites can be used in such clauses with certain types of hypothetical future reference if I should lose or should I lose equivalent to if I lose if you would might could stop doing that usually used as a form of request Sentences with the verb wish and expressions of wish using if only follow similar patterns to the if clauses that are referred to above when they have counterfactual present or past reference When they express a desired event in the near future the modal would is used I wish you would visit me If only he would give me a sign For more information see English conditional sentences Second person singular formsEarly Modern English often distinguished between second person plural you or ye and second person singular thou Rather as English verbs other than modal auxiliaries agree with third person singular subjects in today s English Early Modern English verbs in general modal auxiliaries included agreed with a second person subject that was distinctively singular There was no such agreement with instances of you or ye that happened to have singular reference Examples from Shakespeare are shown below Second person singular thou forms Plain present Preteritecan canst couldstdare darest durstmay mayst mayest mightstmust mustneed needestought oughtestshall shalt shouldstwill wilt wouldstThou canst not then be false to any man Hamlet I 3 Couldst thou save nothing King Lear III 4 Arrest me foolish fellow if thou darest Comedy of Errors IV 1 Durst thou have look d upon him being awake And hast thou kill d him sleeping Midsummer Night s Dream III 2 Safe mayst thou wander safe return again Cymbeline III 5 If thou mayest discern by that which is left of him what he is fetch me to the sight of him Winter s Tale III 3 Then mightst thou speak then mightst thou tear thy hair Romeo and Juliet III 3 thou must know the king is full of grief Winter s Tale IV 4 thy horse stands behind the hedge when thou needest him there thou shalt find him Henry IV Part 1 II 2 thou oughtest not to let thy horse wear a cloak Henry VI Part 2 IV 7 Wherefore shouldst thou pity her Two Gentlemen of Verona IV 4 Wilt thou be lord of all the world Antony and Cleopatra II 7 Why whither Adam wouldst thou have me go As You Like It II 3 Replacements for defective formsAs noted above English modal verbs are defective in that they do not have any untensed form or for some preterite form However in many cases expressions can carry the same meaning as the modal and be used to supply the missing forms The modals can and could expressing ability can be replaced by be able to with the appropriate inflection of be The modals may and might expressing permission can be replaced by be allowed to again with the appropriate inflection of be The modal must in most meanings can be replaced by have to with the appropriate inflection of have When used for futurity will can be replaced by be going to with the appropriate inflection of be The modals should and ought to might be replaced by be supposed to again with the appropriate inflection of be Weak formsMost of the modals have negative inflected forms can t won t etc Although they started as weak forms contractions they are no longer so Genuine contractions are can ˈkaen ken could ˈkʊd ked shall ˈʃael ʃel should ˈʃʊd ʃed will ˈwɪl wel l would ˈwʊd wed d had better ˈhaed ˈbɛteɹ hed ˈbɛteɹ d ˈbɛteɹ ˈbɛteɹ When shall and should are first person replacements for will and would they too may take the weak forms ll and d citation needed A combination like should have is normally reduced to ʃʊd h ev or just ʃʊde shoulda Also ought to can become ɔːte oughta See weak and strong forms in English Effect of negationEither or both of two kinds of negation can apply to a construction using a modal auxiliary verb What is called internal negation is limited to the subordinate clause The difference between He might have overheard you and He might not have overheard you with internal negation is that between It is possible that he overheard you and It is possible that he did not overhear you By contrast what is called external negation applies to the matrix clause The difference between He could have overheard you and He couldn t have overheard you with external negation is that between It is possible that he overheard you and It is not possible that he overheard you Not can be moved into the subordinate clause He might have not overheard you has the same meaning as He might not have overheard you but He could have not overheard you means It is possible that he did not overhear you The two kinds of negation can be combined He can t have not overheard you means It is not possible that he did not overhear you Likewise the difference between You mustn t apologize and You needn t apologize is that the former shows internal negation inverting the necessity the latter external negation negating the necessity Whether negating a modal auxiliary verb brings negation of the subordinate or the matrix clause internal or external negation respectively thus depends on the particular verb which in turn partly depends on the strength of the modality that the verb expresses and there may be other determining factors as well However Negative interrogatives used as questions biased towards a positive answer have external negation irrespective of the strength of the modality A special case is in tags We must stop soon mustn t we Usage of specific verbsCan and could The modal verb can expresses possibility in a dynamic deontic or epistemic sense that is in terms of innate ability permissibility or probability For example Dynamic Ability You needn t struggle with your Tamil when talking to me I can speak English I am capable of speaking English I know how to speak English Existential Most siblings get along at least tolerably well but there can be strong rivalry between them such rivalry does sometimes occur The reasonable acceptable You can be a few minutes late nobody will mind The circumstantially possible Petrol left for months in an unused car can wreck its fuel line This is the result of a predictable chemical process that is not being prevented Deontic Smoking is forbidden anywhere in this building but you can smoke behind the bicycle shed You are permitted to smoke here Epistemic He did the Ironman in under seven hours That can t be true It is impossible for that to be true The preterite form could is used as the past tense or remote conditional form of can in the above meanings see Preterite forms above It is also used to express likelihood We could be in trouble here It is preferable to use could may or might rather than can when expressing likelihood in a particular situation citation needed as opposed to the general case as in the rivalry example above where can or may is used Both can and could can be used to make requests Can could you pass me the cheese means Please pass me the cheese where could is more polite Either can be used with possibly Can could you possibly pass me the cheese Requests with can t may sound impatient Can t you be quiet It is common to use can with verbs of perception such as see hear etc as in I can see a tree Aspectual distinctions can be made such as I could see it ongoing state vs I saw it event citation needed Could have expresses counterfactual past ability or possibility I could have told him if I had seen him I could have told him yesterday but I didn t Can have is less common than may have citation needed Can may be negated by the addition of not ken ˈnɒt analogously to the addition of not to could may will and so forth It can also be negated by inflection its commoner inflected form is can t ˈkɑnt ˈkɑnt or kant in RP General American and General Australian respectively However it has an alternative inflected form cannot ˈkaenɒt Can not and cannot thus differ in placement of the single stress Can not is more formal than can t and does not invert with its subject Can t Cannot we leave now Negated could has the inflected form couldn t Negating can or could is external and negates the matrix clause expressing inability impermissibility or impossibility I can t wear jeans This differs from may or might used to express possibility It can t be true does not mean It may not be true Thus can t or cannot is often used to express disbelief even in possibility as must expresses belief in the certainty When the reference is to the past have is used He can t cannot have done it means It is not possible that he did it compare He must have done it With special stress internal negation is possible I can NOT wear a suit if I wish means I am not compelled to wear a suit if I don t want to May and might The verb may expresses possibility in either an epistemic or deontic sense that is in terms of probability or permissibility For example The mouse may be dead means that it is possible that the mouse perhaps audible until the day before is now dead Trevor may leave if he d prefer to play with his friends means that Trevor is permitted to leave May can have future as well as present reference He may arrive means that it is possible that he will arrive I may go to the mall means that I am considering going to the mall The preterite form might is used as a synonym of may to express a possible circumstance as can could see above It is sometimes said according to whom that might and could express more doubt than may For uses of might in conditional sentences and as a past equivalent to may in such contexts as indirect speech see Preterite forms above May or might can also express concession of a minor point He may be taller than me but he s certainly not stronger could mean While I d agree that he is taller than me that is unimportant as he s certainly not stronger May can indicate permission for present or future actions or be a polite directive You may go now Might used in this way is milder You might go now if you feel like it Similarly May I use your phone is a request for permission Might I use your phone would be more hesitant or polite A less common use of may is optative to express a wish as in May you live long and happy see also English subjunctive May have indicates uncertainty about a past circumstance whereas might have can either have that meaning or refer to possibilities that did not occur but could have see also conditional sentences above She may have eaten the cake The speaker does not know whether she did She might have eaten the cake The speaker either does not know whether she did or knows that she did not eat cake but that her eating it would have been possible May have is used for possibility not permission although the second sense of might have might sometimes imply permission The inflected form mayn t is obsolete The inflected form mightn t mostly appears in the tags of tag questions It might snow tonight mightn t it and in other questions expressing doubt Mightn t I come in if I took my boots off The result of negating may or might depends on whether the interpretation is epistemic about likelihood or deontic about permission In epistemic senses the negation is internal of the subordinate clause There may not be a vote on it this week In deontic senses the negation is normally external You may not go to the party unless you finish your homework but with special stress internal negation is possible I may NOT attend church if I wish meaning I have permission not to attend church Shall and should The verb shall is used in some varieties of English in place of will when the subject is first person I shall we shall With second and third person subjects shall indicates a directive or prophecy Cinderella you shall go to the ball It is often used in writing laws and specifications Those convicted of violating this law shall be imprisoned for a term of not less than three years The device shall be able to operate within a normal temperature range Shall is sometimes used in requests for advice or confirmation of a suggestion Shall I read now What shall we wear Should is sometimes used as a first person equivalent to would in its conditional and future in the past uses in the same way that shall can replace will Should is also used for a protasis with future reference either with the preposition if If you should meet her please give her this or with subject auxiliary inversion Should you meet her please give her this Should is often used to describe an expected or recommended act or state It can be used to give advice or to describe normative behavior though without such strong obligatory force as must or have to Thus You should never lie describes a social or ethical norm It can also express what is expected This should work In these uses it is equivalent to ought Both shall and should can be used with have shall should have done in their role as first person equivalents of will and would thus to form future perfect or conditional perfect structures Also shall have may express an order with perfective aspect You shall have finished your duties by nine o clock When should is used in that way it usually expresses something that would have been expected at some time in the past but did not in fact happen or is not known to have happened I should have done that yesterday It would have been expedient or expected of me to do that yesterday The negative inflections are shan t and shouldn t Negating should negates the subordinate clause the negation is internal You shouldn t use botox To negate the meaning of I should one may useI ought not to or I am not supposed to As for any modal auxiliary a negative interrogative Shouldn t you check your credit card statement instead negates the matrix clause Will and would Will often expresses futurity The next meeting will be held on Thursday Since this is an expression of time rather than modality constructions with will or sometimes shall see Shall and should above are often called the future tense For those speakers who for first person subjects I we use shall to express futurity the use of will for these indicates particular resolve Future events are also sometimes described with the present tense see Uses of English verb forms or using the going to construction Will can express habitual aspect or dynamic modality for example He will make mistakes in which will is usually stressed somewhat may mean that he seems often to make them Will also has these uses as a modal It can express strong probability with present time reference as in That will be John at the door It can be used to give an indirect order as in You will do it right now Modal uses of the preterite form would include Would is used in some conditional sentences vague Expression of politeness as in I would like to to politely state a preference and Would you be so kind as to do this for Please do this As a tense marker would is used for Future of the past as in I knew I would graduate two years later Would is the past form of future will as described above under Preterite forms It is sometimes replaced by should in the first person in the same way that will is replaced by shall As an aspect marker would is used for Expression of habitual aspect in the past as in Back then I would eat early and would walk to school Both will and would can be used with have will have would have to form the future perfect and conditional perfect forms already referred to or to express perfective aspect in their other meanings e g there will have been an arrest order expressing strong probability The negative inflections are won t and wouldn t For contracted forms of will and would themselves see Weak forms above Negating will or would is internal and negates the subordinate clause I won t be surprised if it rains means I will be unsurprised if it rains But as for any modal auxiliary a negative interrogative Won t Wouldn t we submit them in person negates the matrix clause Would rather would sooner and would as soon Would rather would sooner or would as soon can take as its complement either a bare infinitival clause She would rather go herself or a declarative content clause She would rather that I went They are PPIs although I would rather not catch the virus with negation of the clause that is subordinate to would rather is idiomatic I wouldn t rather catch the virus with negation of the matrix clause is distinctly strange Whether its reference is to past present or future the declarative content clause complement can use the preterite I d rather you hadn t told her that past counterfactual I d rather you didn t tell her that present future I d rather you didn t tell her that when you meet her future Must Must differs from the central modal auxiliary verbs in lacking a preterite It expresses obligation or necessity You must use this form We must try to escape It can also express a conclusion reached by indirect evidence e g Sue must be at home When used with have and a past participle must has only an epistemic flavor Sue must have left means that the speaker concludes that Sue has left To express obligation or necessity in the past had to or some other synonym must be used The negative inflection of must is mustn t Negation of must is internal negating the subordinate clause You must not mustn t drive after smoking a joint means that not driving is what you must do But as for any modal auxiliary a negative interrogative Mustn t we hide the dope negates the matrix clause To express the lack of requirement or obligation the negative of have to or need see below can be used You don t have to do this You needn t do this Negated must is not commonly used in an epistemic sense where it is common to use can t It can t be here Sue can t have left instead Mustn t can nonetheless be used as a simple negative of must in tag questions and other questions expressing doubt We must do it mustn t we Mustn t he be in the operating room by this stage Must and needs can occur in sequence Hendrik Poutsma writes that The force of must notably that of representing the subject under pressure of an overmastering desire is often emphasized by needs Examples of the pair are The control of the Firm needs must stay within the Family It needs must be said that any observation made in this order shall not be taken as observations on merits thinking it through to its ultimate logical consequences must needs lead to insoluble contradictions the Constitution envisions and by extension the country as a whole must needs have a truly high minded Supreme CourtOught Ought differs from the central modal auxiliary verbs both in taking as its complement a to infinitival rather than a bare infinitival clause compare He should go with He ought to go and in lacking a preterite Ought is used with meanings similar to those of should expressing expectation or requirement The reduced pronunciation of ought to see Weak forms above is sometimes spelt oughtta Ought can be used with have in the same way as should plus intervening to You ought to have done that earlier Ought not to or oughtn t to can be substituted for shouldn t Had better has a similar meaning to should and ought for a deontic meaning expressing recommended or expedient behavior You ought to should had better arrive on time but not other perhaps than jokingly for an epistemic meaning The Sun ought to should had better come out soon Negating ought is internal and negates the subordinate clause I ought not to have a third glassful means that what I ought to do is decline the glassful But as for any modal auxiliary a negative interrogative Oughtn t we to offer cola as well as beer negates the matrix clause The use of ought as a lexical verb as in They didn t ought to go is generally thought of as restricted to nonstandard dialects but has been described as also sometimes found in informal standard usage Lexical ought with the dummy operator do has been condemned in British usage handbooks What this censure suggests is that lexical ought with periphrastic do is a well established usage in colloquial British English Data from a corpus of American and British spoken and written English of the 1980s and 1990s show that ought not to oughtn t to both modal auxiliary and didn t ought to are rare in both American and British English whether written or spoken I don t think you ought to and similar are commonly used instead In interrogatives ought does not appear in American conversation or fiction or in British conversation In British fiction the modal auxiliary is used Ought we to not lexical ought with do support Need As a modal auxiliary verb need is a negative polarity item appearing in negative contexts and other contexts that do not affirm Thus We need not remain silent Need we remain silent We need remain silent Like must modal need has no preterite form Although as a modal auxiliary verb need takes a bare infinitival clause complement He needn t overhaul it lexical verb need can take either an object complement He needs my help or a to infinitival clause complement He needs to overhaul it optionally with a subject He needs me to overhaul it Negation of need is external negating the matrix clause You needn t apply again does not say that there is a need not to apply merely that there is no need to apply So although the verb must can usually be substituted for the modal verb need mustn t usually cannot be substituted for needn t Exceptionally the pair are synonymous in polar interrogatives Needn t mustn t we pay now Modal need can also be used with have Need I have done that It is most commonly used here in the negative meaning that an action was from the present perspective not in fact necessary You needn t have left that tip Data from a corpus of American and British spoken and written English of the 1980s and 1990s show that for negative constructions involving need modal auxiliary need is more common in written English both American and British but is less common than lexical need in British English conversation and unused in American English conversation 163 In both American and British English interrogative constructions that require subject auxiliary inversion show do support of lexical need much more commonly than inversion of auxiliary need moreover many of what instances there are of auxiliary need are of fixed formulas Need I say more etc For needs must and must needs see under must Dare As a modal auxiliary verb dare is another negative negative polarity item appearing in negative contexts and other contexts that do not affirm Dare is now much more common as a lexical verb Lexical verb dare takes a to infinitival clause as its complement I didn t dare to answer her and this may have a subject He dared me to dive from a higher board modal dare a bare infinitival clause complement Negation of dare is external what is negated is the matrix clause She dare not attempt it means She doesn t dare to attempt it Examples of the use of modal auxiliary dare followed by equivalents using lexical dare where appropriate If he dare try it he may succeed If he dares to try it he may succeed If he dared try it he might succeed If he dared to try it he might succeed Dare he do it Does he dare to do it Dared he do it Did he dare to do it I daren t or dare not try I don t dare to try I dared not try I didn t dare to try How dare you formulaic expression of outrage I dare say or daresay it s true Another formulaic expression here exceptionally in an affirmative context unexpected for an NPI However its affirmative context causes He dared speak up to be ungrammatical Although seemingly obsolete in the 21st century daredn t was in use in the early 20th I daredn t hurry said Mr Colclough setting us down at the station I was afraid of a skid One s so safe with such a son to con her Through all the noises and through all the press Boys daredn t squirt tormenters on her dress 83 Dared has supplanted an earlier preterite form durst Examples The former retired with cattle and other booty to their mountains whither they knew well the Lowlanders durst not follow them 85 Other debts I durst not face 86 dizzy church crowned central peak that time durst not touch 87 Durst had a negative inflected form durstn t Examples I m under authority you know and durstn t overstep 88 the boat where I durstn t kick for fear of poking my feet through the bottom 89 I durstn t go home to tell Mother Pring 90 Lexical verb dare is close to an NPI She dared to speak up is much less likely than She didn t dare to speak up And the lexical modal distinction is blurred lexical dare commonly occurs in non affirmative contexts without to She wouldn t dare ask her father and it also can be stranded as in She ought to have asked for a raise but she didn t dare Data from a corpus of American and British spoken and written English of the 1980s and 1990s show that dare modal or lexical is infrequent and is found chiefly in fiction and British English conversation In negative constructions in American fiction lexical dare is more common In Britain modal auxiliary dare is Further negation of preterite dared is rare In both American and British English interrogative constructions that require subject auxiliary inversion show inversion of auxiliary dare much more commonly than do support of lexical dare however many of the instances here of auxiliary dare are of fixed formulas How dare you etc Used Used just is far more commonly encountered as a lexical than as an auxiliary verb particularly for younger or American speakers The plain form use sometimes spelt used of the lexical verb is seen in Did you use to play tennis Although rare its preterite perfect had used is attested The first of The Cambridge Grammar of the English Language s five criteria for modal auxiliary verbs is irrelevant to auxiliary verb used which fails the last three The auxiliary verb is also semantically quite distinct from the modal auxiliaries the meaning it expresses is aspectual not modal The Cambridge Grammar does not class auxiliary used as a modal auxiliary verb For more about use see English auxiliary verbs Modal idioms with have The verb had in the expression had better lacks any untensed form Tomorrow you will have better concentrate I ve had better work hard since I started We re having better concentrate and hence is sometimes classed as a modal idiom a semi modal or an emerging or quasi modal verb Negating had better whether by had better not or by hadn t better normally negates the subordinate clause it is internal You d better not stick around However as for any modal auxiliary a negative interrogative Hadn t we better scarper before the police come negates the matrix clause Had best and had rather similarly lack any untensed form Had best is much less common than had better Since had rather and would rather are both likely to be realized as d rather it is rarely easy to decide which of the pair is being used Hendrik Poutsma adds I had as lief or lieve although now antiquated and mostly replaced by I had as soon has never fallen completely into disuse The shortening of had to d has given rise to would being sometimes substituted for it Frequency of useDuring the second half of the 20th century the frequencies of use of both the modal auxiliary verbs and of alternatives to them showed considerable change A comparison of the frequencies in the British corpora LOB and FLOB with material from 1961 and 1991 respectively and of those in the American corpora Brown and Frown 1961 and 1992 material respectively shows Changes in frequencies of use of English modals and quasi modals Modals Quasi modalsBrE AmE BrE AmEcan 2 2 1 5 be going to 1 2 51 6 could 2 4 6 8 be to 17 2 40 1 may 17 4 32 4 had better 26 0 17 1 might 15 1 4 5 have got to 34 1 15 6 must 29 0 34 4 have to 9 0 1 1 need 40 2 12 5 need to 249 1 123 2 ought to 44 2 30 0 be supposed to 113 6 6 3 shall 43 7 43 8 want to 18 5 70 9 should 11 8 13 5 will 2 7 11 1 would 11 0 6 1 Percentage changes shown in parentheses come with x2 values of greater than 0 05 they are of less statistical significance A study of modal auxiliary verbs and quasi modals in American British and Australian examples given equal weight of a variety of genres of written and spoken English in the 1990s found that the totals were Frequencies of use of English modals and quasi modals Modals Quasi modalscan 7663 be able to 889could 3557 be about to 124may 2261 be bound to 27might 1499 be going to 2721must 1367 be supposed to 171need 56 be to 371ought to 126 had better 89shall 343 have got to 705should 2432 have to 2827will 8505 need to 716would 7775 want to 1897Total 35584 Total 10537 Commenting on a different but similar set of figures Longman Grammar of Spoken and Written English observes of ought need dare and use jus In view of the considerable attention given to these marginal auxiliaries in grammatical descriptions of English and English language teaching materials it is worth noting how rare they are particularly in negative and interrogative auxiliary constructions DeductionIn English modal verbs as must have got and could can are used to express deduction and contention The modal verbs state how sure the speaker is about something You re shivering you must be cold Someone must have taken the key it is not here I didn t order ten books This has to be a mistake These aren t mine they ve got to be yours It can t be a burglar All the doors and windows are locked Modals at the head of chainsThe verb governed by the modal may be another auxiliary necessarily one that can appear in plain form this includes be and have but not another modal except in the non standard cases described below under Double modals Hence a modal may introduce a chain of verb forms in which the other auxiliaries express properties such as aspect and voice as in He must have been given a new job If infinitival to is regarded as an auxiliary verb then longer chains are possible as in He must have been encouraged to try to serve tea Double modals In Standard English since a modal auxiliary verb is followed by a verb in its plain form which modals lack it cannot be followed by a second modal auxiliary verb Might have is grammatical have is here the plain form of a non modal verb but might must is not However what appear to be sequences of modal auxiliary verbs occur Might could must can might oughta might would must could could oughta might should may can should oughta might can may could would oughta might will may will may should are some of the 76 combinations attested in Southern American English Those with might as the first modal are easily the most common and might could is the most common of them all Longer sequences such as might should oughta are also attested In Britain by contrast the most common is would might although commonness is relative double modal auxiliary verbs occur only rarely in spontaneous speech even in varieties in which they are known to be used The syntactic status of sequences such as might could and would might is unclear One possibility is that might has been reanalysed by the speaker as an English adverbs and thus be functioning as an adjunct Two rules from different grammatical models supposedly disallow the construction Phrase structure grammar sees the surface clause as allowing only one modal verb and main verb analysis dictates that modal verbs occur in finite forms Comparison with other Germanic languagesMany English modals have cognates in other Germanic languages if often with different meanings Unlike the English modals however such verbs are not generally defective In German mogen mussen konnen sollen wollen cognates of may must can shall and will Although German shares five modal verbs with English their meanings are often quite different Mogen does not mean be allowed but may as epistemic modal and like as a normal verb followed by a noun phrase It can be followed by an infinitive with the meaning of have a desire to do something Wollen means will only in the sense of want to do something and is not used for future reference for which werden is used instead Mussen konnen and sollen are used similarly to English must can and shall The negation of mussen is a literal one in German not an inverse one as in English German ich muss I must means I need to do something and ich muss nicht literally I must not accordingly means I don t need to do something In English have to do something behaves the same way whereas English must expresses an interdiction when negated brauchen need is sometimes used like a modal verb especially negated Er braucht nicht kommen He need not come In Dutch mogen moeten kunnen zullen willen cognates of may must can shall and will In Danish matte kunne ville skulle cognates of may must can will shall They generally have the same meanings as in English with the exception of ville which usually means want to do something but can also mean will In Swedish ma past tense matte masta kunna vilja ska ll cognates of may might must can will shall Their meanings generally correspond to those in English with the exception of vilja which means want to do something Since modal verbs in other Germanic languages are not defective the problem of double modals see above does not arise the second modal verb in such a construction simply takes the infinitive form like for any verb in the same position Compare the following translations of English I want to be able to dance all of which translate literally as I want can dance except the German as I want dance can German Ich will tanzen konnen Dutch Ik wil kunnen dansen Danish Jeg vil kunne danse Swedish Jag vill kunna dansa See alsoTense aspect mood Invariant auxiliariesNotesAlthough there are non auxiliary modal verbs in English such as require and oblige the aim of brevity causes this article often to use modal or modal verb to mean modal auxiliary verb The plain form of a verb is exemplified by beware and the form be of the verb be You should beware of the dog You should be careful It is distinguished from the plain present form of the verb as exemplified by am are and were For any verb other than be that is not defective the plain and plain present forms are identical in pronunciation and spelling More precisely Does there exist a lexical verb with the same spelling and pronunciation that is synonymous or could be said to have an auxiliary or copular function Ignored here is any lexical verb will meaning exert one s will in an attempt to compel can meaning insert into cans etc that is unlikely to be mistaken for the auxiliary verb T here is evidence that for some speakers of Standard English may and might have diverged to the extent that they are no longer inflectional forms of a single lexeme but belong to distinct lexemes may and might each of which like must lacks a preterite An NPI rare for speakers of Standard American English Pronounced just rhyming with roost Auxiliary verb form used should be distinguished from the homonymous adjective used as in I ve got very used to it The homographic verb form used juzd rhyming with refused is lexical only For many speakers of Standard English especially younger ones use jus is exclusively a lexical verb This article uses asterisks to indicate ungrammatical expressions As an example She can cans try means that although She can try is grammatical She cans try is not ReferencesHuddleston amp Pullum 2002 p 104 Aarts 2011 pp 83 85 Quirk et al 1985 page needed Palmer 1988 pp 242 248 Huddleston amp Pullum 2002 p 109 Zandvoort 1975 p 85 Huddleston amp Pullum 2002 p 115 Huddleston amp Pullum 2002 pp 94 107 Huddleston amp Pullum 2002 p 91 Huddleston amp Pullum 2002 pp 74 75 Huddleston amp Pullum 2002 pp 109 115 Huddleston amp Pullum 2002 pp 109 113 Huddleston amp Pullum 2002 pp 108 110 Palmer 2001 p 33 Palmer 1965 page needed Levine 2012 pp 187 204 Quirk et al 1985 pp 135 136 Palmer 1988 p 26 Warner 1993 p 11 Huddleston amp Pullum 2002 pp 108 109 Aarts 2011 pp 280 298 Huddleston amp Pullum 2002 p 108 Collins 2009 p 14 Quirk et al 1985 pp 138 140 Huddleston amp Pullum 2002 pp 109 111 Aarts 2011 pp 298 301 Quirk et al 1985 pp 138 140 Palmer 1988 p 170 Aarts 2011 pp 273 274 Palmer 1988 pp 170 171 Quirk et al 1985 pp 141 143 Aarts 2011 pp 303 304 Huddleston amp Pullum 2002 pp 108 113 Palmer 1988 pp 128 131 141 143 Warner 1993 p 46 Huddleston amp Pullum 2002 pp 111 114 Aarts 2011 pp 301 302 304 305 Palmer 1988 p 106 Quirk et al 1985 pp 143 146 OED 1989 Huddleston amp Pullum 2002 p 1613 Huddleston amp Pullum 2002 p 1615 Huddleston amp Pullum 2002 p 175 Huddleston amp Pullum 2002 pp 175 1216 Huddleston amp Pullum 2002 p 176 Huddleston amp Pullum 2002 p 205 Huddleston amp Pullum 2002 p 185 Huddleston amp Pullum 2002 p 184 Huddleston amp Pullum 2002 p 183 Huddleston amp Pullum 2002 p 181 Huddleston amp Pullum 2002 p 865 Huddleston amp Pullum 2002 p 940 Huddleston amp Pullum 2002 pp 768 770 Huddleston amp Pullum 2002 p 1611 Huddleston amp Pullum 2002 pp 180 184 204 804 805 Huddleston amp Pullum 2002 pp 856 944 Huddleston amp Pullum 2002 pp 180 182 184 204 Koltai 2013 Huddleston amp Pullum 2002 pp 187 204 Fleischman 1982 pp 86 97 Comrie 1985 pp 21 47 48 Ultralingua n d StudySpanish n d Huddleston amp Pullum 2002 pp 193 205 Huddleston amp Pullum 2002 p 829 Huddleston amp Pullum 2002 pp 1003 1004 Huddleston amp Pullum 2002 pp 180 182 204 205 Poutsma 1929 p 61 Kapoor 2013 Daily Excelsior 2018 von Mises 2020 Adler 2018 Huddleston amp Pullum 2002 pp 187 205 Quirk et al 1985 p 140 Greenbaum 1996 p 155 Lee amp Collins 2004 p 502 Biber et al 1999 p 165 Biber et al 1999 p 218 Huddleston amp Pullum 2002 p 180 Biber et al 1999 p 217 Huddleston amp Pullum 2002 p 196 Bennett 1971 p 186 Masefield 1912 page needed Huddleston amp Pullum 2002 p 110n Worsaae 1852 page needed Yonge 1875 page needed Lovecraft 2022 page needed Ballantyne 2007 page needed Fenn 2007 page needed Blackmore 2021 page needed Huddleston amp Pullum 2002 pp 110 111 Huddleston amp Pullum 2002 p 110 Biber et al 1999 p 163 Biber et al 1999 p 164 Biber et al 1999 pp 217 218 Huddleston amp Pullum 2002 p 92 Collins 2009 p 16 Leech et al 2009 pp 105 106 Krug 2009 p 332 Huddleston amp Pullum 2002 pp 196 205 Van der Auwera Noel amp Van linden 2013 p 6 Poutsma 1929 p 158 Collins 2009 p 7 Mair amp Leech 2006 pp 327 328 Collins 2009 p 5 Biber et al 1999 p 219 Learn English n d Murphy 2013 Yule 2006 p 40 Morin amp Grieve 2024 pp 11 13 Hasty 2012 pp 1717 1718 Coats 2023 pp 11 17 Coats 2023 p 2 Lebedeva amp Orlova 2019 pp 71 84 Di Paolo 1989 p 195 Works cited Aarts Bas 2011 Oxford Modern English Grammar Oxford Oxford University Press ISBN 978 0 19 953319 0 Adler Bruce K 15 October 2018 How to depoliticize the US Supreme Court Hippo Reads Retrieved 19 December 2023 Ballantyne R M 2007 1876 Under the Waves Diving in Deep Waters via Project Gutenberg Bennett Arnold 1971 1907 The Death of Simon Fuge The Grim Smile of the Five Towns Harmondsworth Middlesex Penguin ISBN 978 0 14 000519 6 via Internet Archive Biber Douglas Johansson Stig Leech Geoffrey Conrad Susan Finegan Edward 1999 Longman Grammar of Spoken and Written English Harlow Essex Pearson Education ISBN 978 0 582 23725 4 via Internet Archive Blackmore R D 2021 1895 Slain by the Doones via Project Gutenberg Coats Steven December 2023 Double modals in contemporary British and Irish speech PDF English Language and Linguistics 27 4 693 718 doi 10 1017 S1360674323000126 Collins Peter 2009 Modals and Quasi modals in English Language and Computers Studies in Practical Linguistics 67 Amsterdam Rodopi ISBN 978 90 420 2532 5 Comrie Bernard 1985 Tense Cambridge Textbooks in Linguistics Cambridge University Press ISBN 978 0 521 28138 6 HC rejects quashing of FIR in multi crore FCI scam Daily Excelsior 3 August 2018 Retrieved 19 December 2023 Di Paolo Marianna 1989 Double modals as single lexical items American Speech 64 3 195 224 doi 10 2307 455589 JSTOR 455589 Fenn George Manville 2007 1891 To the West via Project Gutenberg Fleischman Suzanne 1982 The Future in Thought and Language Diachronic Evidence from Romance Cambridge University Press ISBN 978 0 521 24389 6 Greenbaum Sidney 1996 The Oxford English Grammar Oxford Oxford University Press ISBN 978 0 19 861250 6 Hasty J Daniel 2012 We might should oughta take a second look at this A syntactic re analysis of double modals in Southern United States English Lingua 122 14 1716 1738 doi 10 1016 j lingua 2012 09 005 Huddleston Rodney Pullum Geoffrey K 2002 The Cambridge Grammar of the English Language Cambridge Cambridge University Press ISBN 978 0 521 43146 0 Kapoor Virendra 26 January 2013 Jaipur Shivir had one point agenda The Sunday Guardian Retrieved 19 December 2023 Koltai Anastasia February 21 2013 English Grammar Usage of Shall vs Should with Examples My English Teacher Krug Manfred 2009 Modality and the history of English adhortatives In Salkie Raphael Busuttil Pierre van der Auwera Johan eds Modality in English Theory and Description Topics in English Linguistics 58 Berlin Mouton de Gruyter pp 315 347 ISBN 978 3 11 019634 4 Modals deduction present Learn English British Council n d Archived from the original on 2014 12 15 Lebedeva Irina S Orlova Svetlana N 2019 Semantics and pragmatics of the double modal might could Training Language and Culture 3 2 71 84 doi 10 29366 2019tlc 3 2 5 via Cyberleninka Lee Jackie F K Collins Peter 2004 On the usage of have dare need ought and used to in Australian English and Hong Kong English World Englishes 23 4 501 513 doi 10 1111 j 0083 2919 2004 00374 x Leech Geoffrey Hundt Marianne Mair Christian Smith Nicholas 2009 Change in Contemporary English A Grammatical Study Cambridge Cambridge University Press ISBN 978 0 521 86722 1 Levine Robert D 2012 Auxiliaries To s company Journal of Linguistics 48 1 187 203 doi 10 1017 S002222671100034X ISSN 0022 2267 Lovecraft H P 2022 1924 The Festival via Project Gutenberg von Mises Ludwig 7 July 2020 1944 Mises on the Velocity of Circulation Mises Wire Retrieved 19 December 2023 Mair Christian Leech Geoffrey N 2006 Current changes in English syntax In Aarts Basil McMahon April eds Handbook of English Linguistics Oxford Blackwell pp 318 342 ISBN 978 1 4051 1382 3 Masefield John 1912 The Widow in the Bye Street London Sidgwick amp Jackson OCLC 330395 via Gutenberg Project Morin Cameron Grieve Jack 2024 The semantics sociolinguistics and origins of double modals in American English New insights from social media PLOS ONE 19 1 e0295799 Bibcode 2024PLoSO 1995799M doi 10 1371 journal pone 0295799 PMC 10807846 PMID 38265988 Murphy Chris 18 June 2013 Modals Deduction Past EC English Language Centres need Oxford English Dictionary Second ed 1989 Palmer Frank Robert 2001 Mood and Modality Cambridge Cambridge University Press ISBN 978 0 521 80479 0 Palmer F R 1988 The English Verb 2nd ed London Longman ISBN 978 0 582 01470 1 Palmer Frank Robert 1965 A Linguistic Study of the English Verb London Longmans Poutsma H 1929 A Grammar of Late Modern English PDF Vol 1 2nd ed Groningen P Noordhoff p 158 OCLC 697774306 via Internet Archive Quirk Randolph Greenbaum Sidney Leech Geoffrey Svartvik Jan 1985 A Comprehensive Grammar of the English Language London Longman ISBN 978 0 582 51734 9 The Conditional Tense Study Spanish n d Conditional tense UltraLingua Online Dictionary amp Grammar n d Archived from the original on 2009 10 11 Van der Auwera Johan Noel Dirk Van linden An 2013 Had better d better and better Diachronic and transatlantic variation PDF In Marin Arrese Juana I Carretero Marta Arus Jorge Van der Auwera Johan eds English Modality Core Periphery and Evidentiality Topics in English Linguistics 81 Berlin De Gruyter Mouton pp 119 154 ISBN 978 3 11 028632 8 Retrieved 29 November 2023 via University of Liege Warner Anthony R 1993 English Auxiliaries Structure and History Cambridge Studies in Linguistics 66 Cambridge Cambridge University Press ISBN 978 0 521 30284 5 Worsaae J J A 1852 An Account of the Danes and Norwegians in England Scotland and Ireland London John Murray via Project Gutenberg Yonge Charlotte M 1875 The Pillars of the House or Under Wode under Rode Vol 2 London Macmillan via Project Gutenberg Yule George 2006 Oxford Practice Grammar Advanced Oxford University Press ISBN 978 0 19 432754 1 Zandvoort R W 1975 A Handbook of English Grammar 7th ed London Longman ISBN 978 0 582 55339 2 Look up Appendix English modal verbs in Wiktionary the free dictionary