![Confabulation](https://www.english.nina.az/image-resize/1600/900/web/wikipedia.jpg)
Confabulation is a memory error consisting of the production of fabricated, distorted, or misinterpreted memories about oneself or the world. It is generally associated with certain types of brain damage (especially aneurysm in the anterior communicating artery) or a specific subset of dementias. While still an area of ongoing research, the basal forebrain is implicated in the phenomenon of confabulation. People who confabulate present with incorrect memories ranging from subtle inaccuracies to surreal fabrications, and may include confusion or distortion in the temporal framing (timing, sequence or duration) of memories. In general, they are very confident about their recollections, even when challenged with contradictory evidence.
Confabulation occurs when individuals mistakenly recall false information, without intending to deceive. Brain damage, dementia, and anticholinergic toxidrome can cause this distortion. Two types of confabulation exist: provoked and spontaneous, with two distinctions: verbal and behavioral. Verbal statements, false information, and the patient's unawareness of the distortion are all associated with this phenomenon. Personality structure also plays a role in confabulation.
Numerous theories have been developed to explain confabulation. Neuropsychological theories suggest that cognitive dysfunction causes the distortion. Self-identity theories posit that people confabulate to preserve themselves. The temporality theory believes that confabulation occurs when an individual cannot place events properly in time. The monitoring and strategic retrieval account theories argue that confabulation arises when individuals cannot recall memories correctly or monitor them after retrieval. The executive control and fuzzy-trace theories also attempt to explain why confabulation happens.
Confabulation can occur with nervous system injuries or illnesses, including Korsakoff's syndrome, Alzheimer's disease, schizophrenia, and traumatic brain injury. It is believed that the right frontal lobe of the brain is damaged, causing false memories. Children are especially susceptible to forced confabulation as they are highly impressionable. Feedback can increase confidence in false memories. In rare cases, confabulation occurs in ordinary individuals.
Different memory tests, including recognition tasks and free recall tasks, can be used to study confabulation. Treatment depends on the underlying cause of the distortion. Ongoing research aims to develop a standard test battery to discern between different types of confabulations, distinguish delusions from confabulations, understand the role of unconscious processes, and identify pathological and nonpathological confabulations.
Description
Confabulation was originally defined as "the emergence of memories of events and experiences which never took place".
Confabulation is distinguished from lying as there is no intent to deceive and the person is unaware the information is false. Although individuals can present blatantly false information, confabulation can also seem to be coherent, internally consistent, and relatively normal.
Most known cases of confabulation are symptomatic of brain damage or dementias, such as aneurysm, Alzheimer's disease, or Wernicke–Korsakoff syndrome (a common manifestation of thiamine deficiency caused by alcohol use disorder). Additionally confabulation often occurs in people with anticholinergic toxidrome when interrogated about bizarre or irrational behaviour.
Confabulated memories of all types most often occur in autobiographical memory and are indicative of a complicated and intricate process that can be led astray at any point during encoding, storage, or recall of a memory. This type of confabulation is commonly seen in Korsakoff's syndrome.
Distinctions
Several forms of confabulation have been distinguished which vary in terms of behaviour, mechanism, and location of brain damage. The most common distinction is between the following two types:
- Provoked confabulations represent a normal response to a faulty memory, are common in both amnesia and dementia, and can become apparent during memory tests.
- Spontaneous confabulations do not occur in response to a cue and seem to be involuntary. They are relatively rare, more common in cases of dementia, and may result from the interaction between frontal lobe pathology and organic amnesia. A subgroup of patients at least occasionally act according to their confabulations betraying a confusion of current reality. These patients are always disoriented about their current role, location, and time. This form has been called behaviorally spontaneous confabulation. It is strongly associated with damage, disconnection, or dysfunction of the posterior medial orbitofrontal cortex (area 13) and appears to have a specific mechanism: a failure of "orbitofrontal reality filtering", a preconscious mechanism normally verifying whether an upcoming thought refers to current reality or not.
Signs and symptoms
Confabulation is associated with several characteristics:
- False verbal statements that may include autobiographical and non-personal information, such as historical facts, fairy-tales, or other aspects of semantic memory.
- The account is normally coherent and is usually drawn from the patient's memory of actual experiences. Very rarely, it is fantastic.
- Both the premise and the details of the account can be false.
- The patient is unaware of the accounts' distortions or inappropriateness, and is not concerned when errors are pointed out.
- There is no hidden motivation behind the account.
- The patient's personality structure may play a role in their readiness to confabulate.
Theories
Theories of confabulation range in emphasis. Some theories propose that confabulations represent a way for memory disabled people to maintain their self-identity. Other theories use neurocognitive links to explain the process of confabulation. Still other theories frame confabulation around the more familiar concept of delusion. Other researchers frame confabulation within the fuzzy-trace theory. Finally, some researchers call for theories that rely less on neurocognitive explanations and more on epistemic accounts.
Neuropsychological theories
The most popular theories of confabulation come from the field of neuropsychology or cognitive neuroscience. Controlled experimental evidence is, however, scarce. Research suggests that confabulation is associated with dysfunction of cognitive processes that control the retrieval from long-term memory. Frontal lobe damage often disrupts this process, preventing the retrieval of information and the evaluation of its output. Furthermore, researchers argue that confabulation is a disorder resulting from failed "reality monitoring/source monitoring" (i.e. deciding whether a memory is based on an actual event or whether it is imagined). Some neuropsychologists suggest that errors in retrieval of information from long-term memory that are made by normal subjects involve different components of control processes than errors made by confabulators. Fantastic confabulations have been attributed to a dysfunction of the Supervisory System, which is believed to be a function of the frontal cortex.
Temporality theory
Support for the temporality account suggests that confabulations occur when an individual is unable to place events properly in time. Thus, an individual might correctly state an action they performed, but say they did it yesterday, when they did it weeks ago. In the Memory, Consciousness, and Temporality Theory, confabulation occurs because of a deficit in temporal consciousness or awareness.
Monitoring theory
Along a similar notion are the theories of reality and source monitoring theories. In these theories, confabulation occurs when individuals incorrectly attribute memories as reality, or incorrectly attribute memories to a certain source. Thus, an individual might claim an imagined event happened in reality, or that a friend told him/her about an event he/she actually heard about on television.
Strategic retrieval account theory
Supporters of the strategic retrieval account suggest that confabulations occur when an individual cannot actively monitor a memory for truthfulness after its retrieval. An individual recalls a memory, but there is some deficit after recall that interferes with the person establishing its falseness.
Executive control theory
Still others propose that all types of false memories, including confabulation, fit into a general memory and executive function model. In 2007, a framework for confabulation was proposed that stated confabulation is the result of two things: Problems with executive control and problems with evaluation. In the executive control deficit, the incorrect memory is retrieved from the brain. In the evaluative deficit, the memory will be accepted as a truth due to an inability to distinguish a belief from an actual memory.
In the context of delusion theories
Recent models of confabulation have attempted to build upon the link between delusion and confabulation. More recently, a monitoring account for delusion, applied to confabulation, proposed both the inclusion of conscious and unconscious processing. The claim was that by encompassing the notion of both processes, spontaneous versus provoked confabulations could be better explained. In other words, there are two ways to confabulate. One is the unconscious, spontaneous way in which a memory goes through no logical, explanatory processing. The other is the conscious, provoked way in which a memory is recalled intentionally by the individual to explain something confusing or unusual.
Fuzzy-trace theory
Fuzzy-trace theory, or FTT, is a concept more commonly applied to the explanation of judgement decisions. According to this theory, memories are encoded generally (gist), as well as specifically (verbatim). Thus, a confabulation could result from recalling the incorrect verbatim memory or from being able to recall the gist portion, but not the verbatim portion, of a memory.
FTT uses a set of five principles to explain false-memory phenomena. Principle 1 suggests that subjects store verbatim information and gist information parallel to one another. Both forms of storage involve the surface content of an experience. Principle 2 shares factors of retrieval of gist and verbatim traces. Principle 3 is based on dual-opponent processes in false memory. Generally, gist retrieval supports false memory, while verbatim retrieval suppresses it. Developmental variability is the topic of Principle 4. As a child develops into an adult, there is obvious improvement in the acquisition, retention, and retrieval of both verbatim and gist memory. However, during late adulthood, there will be a decline in these abilities. Finally, Principle 5 explains that verbatim and gist processing cause vivid remembering. Fuzzy-trace Theory, governed by these 5 principles, has proved useful in explaining false memory and generating new predictions about it.
Epistemic theory
However, not all accounts are so embedded in the neurocognitive aspects of confabulation. Some attribute confabulation to epistemic accounts. In 2009, theories underlying the causation and mechanisms for confabulation were criticized for their focus on neural processes, which are somewhat unclear, as well as their emphasis on the negativity of false remembering. Researchers proposed that an epistemic account of confabulation would be more encompassing of both the advantages and disadvantages of the process.
Presentation
Associated neurological and psychological conditions
Confabulations are often symptoms of various syndromes and psychopathologies in the adult population, including Korsakoff's syndrome, Alzheimer's disease, schizophrenia, and traumatic brain injury.
Wernicke–Korsakoff syndrome is a neurological disorder typically characterized by years of alcohol use disorder characterized by excessive alcohol consumption and a nutritional thiamine deficiency. Confabulation is one salient symptom of this syndrome. A study on confabulation in Korsakoff's patients found that they are subject to provoked confabulation when prompted with questions pertaining to episodic memory, not semantic memory, and when prompted with questions where the appropriate response would be "I don't know." This suggests that confabulation in these patients is "domain-specific." Korsakoff's patients who confabulate are more likely than healthy adults to falsely recognize distractor words, suggesting that false recognition is a "confabulatory behavior."
Alzheimer's disease is a condition with both neurological and psychological components. It is a form of dementia associated with severe frontal lobe dysfunction. Confabulation in individuals with Alzheimer's is often more spontaneous than it is in other conditions, especially in the advanced stages of the disease. Alzheimer's patients demonstrate comparable abilities to encode information as healthy elderly adults, suggesting that impairments in encoding are not associated with confabulation. However, as seen in Korsakoff's patients, confabulation in Alzheimer's patients is higher when prompted with questions investigating episodic memory. Researchers suggest this is due to damage in the posterior cortical regions of the brain, which is a symptom characteristic of Alzheimer's disease.
Schizophrenia is a psychological disorder in which confabulation is sometimes observed. Although confabulation is usually coherent in its presentation, confabulations of schizophrenic patients are often delusional. Researchers have noted that these patients tend to make up delusions on the spot which are often fantastic and become increasingly elaborate with questioning. Unlike patients with Korsakoff's and Alzheimer's, patients with schizophrenia are more likely to confabulate when prompted with questions regarding their semantic memories, as opposed to episodic memory prompting. In addition, confabulation does not appear to be related to any memory deficit in schizophrenic patients. This is contrary to most forms of confabulation. Also, confabulations made by schizophrenic patients often do not involve the creation of new information, but instead involve an attempt by the patient to reconstruct actual details of a past event.
Traumatic brain injury (TBI) can also result in confabulation. Research has shown that patients with damage to the inferior medial frontal lobe confabulate significantly more than patients with damage to the posterior area and healthy controls. This suggests that this region is key in producing confabulatory responses, and that memory deficit is important but not necessary in confabulation. Additionally, research suggests that confabulation can be seen in patients with frontal lobe syndrome, which involves an insult to the frontal lobe as a result of disease or traumatic brain injury (TBI). Finally, rupture of the anterior or posterior communicating artery, subarachnoid hemorrhage, and encephalitis are also possible causes of confabulation.
Location of brain lesions
Confabulation is believed to be a result of damage to the right frontal lobe of the brain. In particular, damage can be localized to the ventromedial frontal lobes and other structures fed by the anterior communicating artery (ACoA), including the basal forebrain, septum, fornix, cingulate gyrus, cingulum, anterior hypothalamus, and head of the caudate nucleus.
Behaviourally spontaneous confabulation may occur in the context of dementia or the Wernicke–Korsakoff syndrome where brain damage is difficult to localise. If it is due to circumscribed brain damage (e.g., after ruptured aneurysm of the anterior communicating artery, traumatic brain injury, stroke), lesions involved the posterior medial orbitofrontal cortex (area 13) or an area directly connected with it.
Developmental differences
While some recent literature has suggested that older adults may be more susceptible than their younger counterparts to have false memories, the majority of research on forced confabulation centers around children. Children are particularly susceptible to forced confabulations based on their high suggestibility. When forced to recall confabulated events, children are less likely to remember that they had previously confabulated these situations, and they are more likely than their adult counterparts to come to remember these confabulations as real events that transpired. Research suggests that this inability to distinguish between past confabulatory and real events is centered on developmental differences in source monitoring. Due to underdeveloped encoding and critical reasoning skills, children's ability to distinguish real memories from false memories may be impaired. It may also be that younger children lack the meta-memory processes required to remember confabulated versus non-confabulated events. Children's meta-memory processes may also be influenced by expectancies or biases, in that they believe that highly plausible false scenarios are not confabulated. However, when knowingly being tested for accuracy, children are more likely to respond, "I don't know" at a rate comparable to adults for unanswerable questions than they are to confabulate. Ultimately, misinformation effects can be minimized by tailoring individual interviews to the specific developmental stage, often based on age, of the participant.
Provoked versus spontaneous confabulations
There is evidence to support different cognitive mechanisms for provoked and spontaneous confabulation. One study suggested that spontaneous confabulation may be a result of an amnesic patient's inability to distinguish the chronological order of events in their memory. In contrast, provoked confabulation may be a compensatory mechanism, in which the patient tries to make up for their memory deficiency by attempting to demonstrate competency in recollection.
Confidence in false memories
Confabulation of events or situations may lead to an eventual acceptance of the confabulated information as true. For instance, people who knowingly lie about a situation may eventually come to believe that their lies are truthful with time. In an interview setting, people are more likely to confabulate in situations in which they are presented false information by another person, as opposed to when they self-generate these falsehoods. Further, people are more likely to accept false information as true when they are interviewed at a later time (after the event in question) than those who are interviewed immediately or soon after the event. Affirmative feedback for confabulated responses is also shown to increase the confabulator's confidence in their response. For instance, in culprit identification, if a witness falsely identifies a member of a line-up, he will be more confident in his identification if the interviewer provides affirmative feedback. This effect of confirmatory feedback appears to last over time, as witnesses will even remember the confabulated information months later.
Among normal subjects
On rare occasions, confabulation can also be seen in normal subjects. It is currently unclear how completely healthy individuals produce confabulations. It is possible that these individuals are in the process of developing some type of organic condition that is causing their confabulation symptoms. It is not uncommon, however, for the general population to display some very mild symptoms of provoked confabulations. Subtle distortions and intrusions in memory are commonly produced by normal subjects when they remember something poorly.
Diagnosis and treatment
Spontaneous confabulations, due to their involuntary nature, cannot be manipulated in a laboratory setting. However, provoked confabulations can be researched in various theoretical contexts. The mechanisms found to underlie provoked confabulations can be applied to spontaneous confabulation mechanisms. The basic premise of researching confabulation comprises finding errors and distortions in memory tests of an individual.
Deese–Roediger–McDermott lists
Confabulations can be detected in the context of the Deese–Roediger–McDermott paradigm by using the Deese–Roediger–McDermott lists. Participants listen to audio recordings of several lists of words centered around a theme, known as the critical word. The participants are later asked to recall the words on their list. If the participant recalls the critical word, which was never explicitly stated in the list, it is considered a confabulation. Participants often have a false memory for the critical word.
Recognition tasks
Confabulations can also be researched by using continuous recognition tasks. These tasks are often used in conjunction with confidence ratings. Generally, in a recognition task, participants are rapidly presented with pictures. Some of these pictures are shown once; others are shown multiple times. Participants press a key if they have seen the picture previously. Following a period of time, participants repeat the task. More errors on the second task, versus the first, are indicative of confusion, representing false memories.
Free recall tasks
Confabulations can also be detected using a free recall task, such as a self-narrative task. Participants are asked to recall stories (semantic or autobiographical) that are highly familiar to them. The stories recalled are encoded for errors that could be classified as distortions in memory. Distortions could include falsifying true story elements or including details from a completely different story. Errors such as these would be indicative of confabulations.
Treatment
Treatment for confabulation is somewhat dependent on the cause or source, if identifiable. For example, treatment of Wernicke–Korsakoff syndrome involves large doses of vitamin B in order to reverse the thiamine deficiency. If there is no known physiological cause, more general cognitive techniques may be used to treat confabulation. A case study published in 2000 showed that (SMT) reduced delusional confabulations. Furthermore, improvements were maintained at a three-month follow-up and were found to generalize to everyday settings. Although this treatment seems promising, more rigorous research is necessary to determine the efficacy of SMT in the general confabulation population.
Research
Although significant gains have been made in the understanding of confabulation in recent years, there is still much to be learned. One group of researchers in particular has laid out several important questions for future study. They suggest more information is needed regarding the neural systems that support the different cognitive processes necessary for normal source monitoring. They also proposed the idea of developing a standard neuropsychological test battery able to discriminate between the different types of confabulations. And there is a considerable amount of debate regarding the best approach to organizing and combining neuro-imaging, pharmacological, and cognitive/behavioral approaches to understand confabulation.
In a recent review article, another group of researchers contemplate issues concerning the distinctions between delusions and confabulation. They question whether delusions and confabulation should be considered distinct or overlapping disorders and, if overlapping, to what degree? They also discuss the role of unconscious processes in confabulation. Some researchers suggest that unconscious emotional and motivational processes are potentially just as important as cognitive and memory problems. Finally, they raise the question of where to draw the line between the pathological and the nonpathological. Delusion-like beliefs and confabulation-like fabrications are commonly seen in healthy individuals. What are the important differences between patients with similar etiology who do and do not confabulate? Since the line between pathological and nonpathological is likely blurry, should we take a more dimensional approach to confabulation? Research suggests that confabulation occurs along a continuum of implausibility, bizarreness, content, conviction, preoccupation, and distress, and impact on daily life.
See also
- Compare with:
- Anosognosia
- Confabulation (neural networks)
- Cryptomnesia
- False memory
- Hallucination
- Hallucination (artificial intelligence)
- Hindsight bias
- Misinformation effect
- Revelation
- Rosy retrospection
- Not to be confused with:
- Scams (swindles):
- Confidence tricks
- Fraud
- Hoaxes
- Gaslighting
- Phishing
- Scams (swindles):
References
- Berrios G E (1998) Confabulations: A Conceptual History. Journal of the History of the Neurosciences. 7: 225-241.
- Pendick, Daniel (Summer 2000). "The truth about confabulation". Memory Loss & the Brain: Newsletter of the Memory Disorders Project. Rutgers University-Newark. Archived from the original on 3 January 2013. Retrieved 7 April 2022.
- Matthews, Paul M.; McClelland, James L. (2010). Nalbantian, Suzanne (ed.). The memory process: neuroscientific and humanistic perspectives. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press. ISBN 978-0-262-01457-1.
- Wernicke, Carl (1900). Grundriss der Psychiatrie in klinischen Vorlesungen. Leipzig: Thieme. p. 139.
- Schnider, Armin (2018). The Confabulating Mind. How the brain creates reality. 2nd edition. Oxford, New York: Oxford University Press. ISBN 9780198789680.
- Moscovitch M. 1995. "Confabulation". In (Eds. Schacter D.L., Coyle J.T., Fischbach G.D., Mesulum M.M. & Sullivan L.G.), Memory Distortion. pp. 226–51. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard University Press.
- Sivolap IuP Damulin IV (2013). "[Wernicke encephalopathy and Korsakoff's psychosis: clinical-pathophysiological correlation, diagnostics and treatment]". Zh Nevrol Psikhiatr Im S S Korsakova. 113 (6 Pt 2): 20–26. PMID 23887463.
- Kopelman, M. D.; Thomson, A. D.; Guerrini, I; Marshall, E. J. (2009). "The Korsakoff Syndrome: Clinical Aspects, Psychology and Treatment". Alcohol and Alcoholism. 44 (2): 148–54. doi:10.1093/alcalc/agn118. PMID 19151162.
- Nahum, L; Bouzerda-Wahlen, A; Guggisberg, A; Ptak, R; Schnider, A (2012). "Forms of confabulation: dissociations and associations". Neuropsychologia. 50 (10): 2524–34. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.06.026. PMID 22781813.
- Kopelman, M. D. (1987). "Two types of confabulation". Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry. 50 (11): 1482–87. doi:10.1136/jnnp.50.11.1482. PMC 1032561. PMID 3694207.
- Metcalf, Kasey; Langdon, Robyn; Coltheart, Max (1 February 2007). "Models of confabulation: A critical review and a new framework". Cognitive Neuropsychology. 24 (1): 23–47. doi:10.1080/02643290600694901. PMID 18416482. S2CID 45885109.
- Gilboa, A. (13 April 2006). "Mechanisms of spontaneous confabulations: a strategic retrieval account". Brain. 129 (6): 1399–1414. doi:10.1093/brain/awl093. PMID 16638795.
- Schnider, A; von Däniken, C; Gutbrod, K (1996). "The mechanisms of spontaneous and provoked confabulations". Brain : A Journal of Neurology. 119 ( Pt 4) (4): 1365–75. doi:10.1093/brain/119.4.1365. PMID 8813298.
- Schnider, A; Ptak, R (1999). "Spontaneous confabulators fail to suppress currently irrelevant memory traces". Nature Neuroscience. 2 (7): 677–81. doi:10.1038/10236. PMID 10404203.
- Schnider, A (2003). "Spontaneous confabulation and the adaptation of thought to ongoing reality". Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 4 (8): 662–71. doi:10.1038/nrn1179. PMID 12894241.
- Schnider, A (2013). "Orbitofrontal reality filtering". Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 7: 67. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00067. PMC 3677127. PMID 23772208.
- Glowinski, Remy; Payman, Vahid; Frencham, Kate (2008). "Confabulation: a spontaneous and fantastic review". Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry. 42 (11): 932–40. doi:10.1080/00048670802415335. PMID 18941957. S2CID 23586700.
- Kopelman, Michael D. (2010). "Varieties of confabulation and delusion". Cognitive Neuropsychiatry. 15 (1–3): 14–37. doi:10.1080/13546800902732830. PMID 19753493. S2CID 19764950.
- Brainerd, C.J.; Reyna, V.F. (1 November 1998). "Fuzzy-Trace Theory and Children's False Memories". Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 71 (2): 81–129. doi:10.1006/jecp.1998.2464. PMID 9843617. S2CID 12290995.
- Bortolotti, Lisa; Cox, Rochelle E. (1 December 2009). "'Faultless' ignorance: Strengths and limitations of epistemic definitions of confabulation". Consciousness and Cognition. 18 (4): 952–65. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2009.08.011. PMID 19773185. S2CID 35121072.
- Baddeley, Alan; Wilson, Barbara (1986). "Amnesia, autobiographical memory, and confabulation". In Rubin, David C. (ed.). Autobiographical Memory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. pp. 225–52. doi:10.1017/CBO9780511558313.020. ISBN 9780511558313.
- Moscovitch M.; Melo B. (1997). "Strategic retrieval and the frontal lobes: evidence from confabulation and amnesia". Neuropsychologia. 35 (7): 1017–34. doi:10.1016/s0028-3932(97)00028-6. PMID 9226662. S2CID 33862514.
- Johnson, M.K. (1991). Reality monitoring: Evidence from confabulation in organic brain disease patients. In G.P. Prigatano & D.L. Schacter (Eds.), Awareness of deficit after brain injury. pp. 176–97. New York: Oxford.
- Burgess, P. W.; Shallice, T (1996). "Confabulation and the control of recollection". Memory. 4 (4): 359–411. doi:10.1080/096582196388906. PMID 8817460.
- Norman, D.A., & Shallice, T. (1980). Attention to action. Willed and automatic control of behavior. University of California San Diego CHIP Report 99. Later published as: Norman, Donald A.; Shallice, Tim (1986). "Attention to Action: Willed and Automatic Control of Behavior". Consciousness and Self-Regulation. pp. 1–18. doi:10.1007/978-1-4757-0629-1_1. ISBN 978-1-4757-0631-4.
- Dalla Barba, Gianfranco; Boissé, Marie-Françoise (2010). "Temporal consciousness and confabulation: Is the medial temporal lobe "temporal"?". Cognitive Neuropsychiatry. 15 (1–3): 95–117. doi:10.1080/13546800902758017. PMID 19750399. S2CID 42447646.
- Kopelman, Michael D. (1 May 1999). "Varieties of False Memory". Cognitive Neuropsychology. 16 (3–5): 197–214. doi:10.1080/026432999380762.
- Turner, Martha; Coltheart, Max (2010). "Confabulation and delusion: A common monitoring framework". Cognitive Neuropsychiatry. 15 (1–3): 346–76. doi:10.1080/13546800903441902. PMID 20043250. S2CID 205768903.
- Brainerd, C. J., & Reyna, V. F. (2002). Fuzzy-Trace Theory and False Memory. Current Directions In Psychological Science (Wiley–Blackwell), 11(5), 164–69.
- Homewood, J; Bond, N. W. (1999). "Thiamin deficiency and Korsakoff's syndrome: Failure to find memory impairments following nonalcoholic Wernicke's encephalopathy". Alcohol. 19 (1): 75–84. doi:10.1016/s0741-8329(99)00027-0. PMID 10487391.
- Dalla Barba G.; Cipolotti L.; Denes G. (1990). "Autobiographical memory loss and confabulation in Korsakoff's syndrome: A case report". Cortex. 26 (4): 525–34. doi:10.1016/s0010-9452(13)80302-4. PMID 2081390. S2CID 4481075.
- Kessels RP, Kortrijk HE, Wester AJ, Nys GM. Confabulation behavior and false memories in Korsakoff's syndrome: role of source memory and executive functioning. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2008 Apr; 62(2) 220–25.
- Damme, Ilse; d'Ydewalle, Géry (2010). "Confabulation versus experimentally induced false memories in Korsakoff patients". Journal of Neuropsychology. 4 (2): 211–30. doi:10.1348/174866409X478231. PMID 19930792.
- Cooper, Janine M.; Shanks, Michael F.; Venneri, Annalena (11 May 2006). "Provoked confabulations in Alzheimer's disease". Neuropsychologia. 44 (10): 1697–1707. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.03.029. PMID 16697019. S2CID 41292107.
- Wing, J. K., Cooper, J. E., Sartorius, N. 1974. The description and classification of psychiatric symptoms: An instruction manual for the PSE and catego system. London: Cambridge University Press.
- Lorente-Rovira, E; Pomarol-Clotet, E; McCarthy, R. A.; Berrios, G. E.; McKenna, P. J. (2007). "Confabulation in schizophrenia and its relationship to clinical and neuropsychological features of the disorder". Psychological Medicine. 37 (10): 1403–12. doi:10.1017/S0033291707000566. PMID 17506924. S2CID 32121170.
- Lorente-Rovira, E; Santos-Gómez, J. L.; Moro, M; Villagrán, J. M.; McKenna, P. J. (1 November 2010). "Confabulation in schizophrenia: A neuropsychological study". Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 16 (6): 1018–26. doi:10.1017/S1355617710000718. hdl:10234/88477. PMID 20630120. S2CID 23194952.
- Turner, Martha S.; Cipolotti, Lisa; Yousry, Tarek A.; Shallice, Tim (1 June 2008). "Confabulation: Damage to a specific inferior medial prefrontal system" (PDF). Cortex. 44 (6): 637–48. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2007.01.002. PMID 18472034. S2CID 17414598.
- Baddeley, A; Wilson, B (1988). "Frontal amnesia and the dysexecutive syndrome". Brain and Cognition. 7 (2): 212–30. doi:10.1016/0278-2626(88)90031-0. PMID 3377900. S2CID 26954876.
- Papagno, C; Baddeley, A (1997). "Confabulation in a dysexecutive patient: Implications for models of retrieval". Cortex. 33 (4): 743–52. doi:10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70731-7. PMID 9444475. S2CID 4484799.
- Baddeley, Alan (1996). "Exploring the Central Executive". Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 49A (1): 5–28. doi:10.1080/713755608. S2CID 54633639.
- Dalla Barba G (1993). "Confabulation: knowledge and recollective experience". Cognitive Neuropsychology. 10 (1): 1–20. doi:10.1080/02643299308253454.
- Alexander, M. P.; Freedman, M (1984). "Amnesia after anterior communicating artery aneurysm rupture". Neurology. 34 (6): 752–57. doi:10.1212/wnl.34.6.752. PMID 6539437. S2CID 34357231.
- Irle, E; Wowra, B; Kunert, H. J.; Hampl, J; Kunze, S (1992). "Memory disturbances following anterior communicating artery rupture". Annals of Neurology. 31 (5): 473–80. doi:10.1002/ana.410310503. PMID 1596082. S2CID 42233017.
- Brainerd, C. J.; Reyna, V. F.; Ceci, S. J. (2008). "Developmental reversals in false memory: A review of data and theory". Psychological Bulletin. 134 (3): 343–82. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.134.3.343. PMID 18444700.
- Shapiro, Lauren R.; Purdy, Telisa L. (2005). "Suggestibility and source monitoring errors: blame the interview style, interviewer consistency, and the child's personality". Applied Cognitive Psychology. 19 (4): 489–506. doi:10.1002/acp.1093.
- Shapiro, Lauren R.; Blackford, Cheryl; Chen, Chiung-Fen (2005). "Eyewitness memory for a simulated misdemeanor crime: the role of age and temperament in suggestibility". Applied Cognitive Psychology. 19 (3): 267–89. doi:10.1002/acp.1089.
- Ackil, Jennifer K.; Zaragoza, Maria S. (1 November 1998). "Memorial consequences of forced confabulation: Age differences in susceptibility to false memories". Developmental Psychology. 34 (6): 1358–72. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.34.6.1358. PMID 9823517.
- Ghetti, Simona; Castelli, Paola; Lyons, Kristen E. (2010). "Knowing about not remembering: developmental dissociations in lack-of-memory monitoring". Developmental Science. 13 (4): 611–21. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00908.x. PMID 20590725.
- Ghetti, Simona; Alexander, Kristen Weede (2004). ""If It Happened, I Would Remember It": Strategic Use of Event Memorability in the Rejection of False Autobiographical Events". Child Development. 75 (2): 542–61. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00692.x. PMID 15056205.
- Roebers, Claudia; Fernandez, Olivia (2002). "The Effects of Accuracy Motivation on Children's and Adults' Event Recall, Suggestibility, and Their Answers to Unanswerable Questions". Journal of Cognition and Development. 3 (4): 415–43. doi:10.1207/S15327647JCD3,4-03.
- Scoboria, Alan; Mazzoni, Giuliana; Kirsch, Irving (2008). ""Don't know" responding to answerable and unanswerable questions during misleading and hypnotic interviews". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied. 14 (3): 255–65. doi:10.1037/1076-898X.14.3.255. PMID 18808279.
- Holliday, Robyn E.; Albon, Amanda J. (2004). "Minimising misinformation effects in young children with cognitive interview mnemonics". Applied Cognitive Psychology. 18 (3): 263–81. doi:10.1002/acp.973.
- Schnider, Armin; von Däniken, Christine; Gutbrod, Klemens (19 February 1996). "The mechanisms of spontaneous and provoked confabulations". Brain. 119 (4): 1365–75. doi:10.1093/brain/119.4.1365. PMID 8813298.
- Pickel, Kerri (2004). "When a lie becomes the truth: The effects of self-generated misinformation on eyewitness memory". Memory. 12 (1): 14–26. doi:10.1080/09658210244000072. PMID 15098618. S2CID 21383980.
- Polage, Danielle C. (2004). "Fabrication deflation? The mixed effects of lying on memory". Applied Cognitive Psychology. 18 (4): 455–65. doi:10.1002/acp.995.
- Pezdek, Kathy; Lam, Shirley T.; Sperry, Kathryn (2009). "Forced confabulation more strongly influences event memory if suggestions are other-generated than self-generated". Legal and Criminological Psychology. 14 (2): 241–52. doi:10.1348/135532508X344773.
- Melnyk, Laura; Bruck, Maggie (2004). "Timing moderates the effects of repeated suggestive interviewing on children's eyewitness memory". Applied Cognitive Psychology. 18 (5): 613–31. doi:10.1002/acp.1013.
- Hafstad, Gertrud Sofie; Memon, Amina; Logie, Robert (2004). "Post-identification feedback, confidence and recollections of witnessing conditions in child witnesses". Applied Cognitive Psychology. 18 (7): 901–12. doi:10.1002/acp.1037.
- Zaragoza, M. S.; Payment, K. E.; Ackil, J. K.; Drivdahl, S. B.; Beck, M. (2001). "Interviewing Witnesses: Forced Confabulation and Confirmatory Feedback Increase False Memories". Psychological Science. 12 (6): 473–77. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00388. PMID 11760134. S2CID 11951759.
- Howe, Mark L.; Cicchetti, Dante; Toth, Sheree L.; Cerrito, Beth M. (1 September 2004). "True and False Memories in Maltreated Children". Child Development. 75 (5): 1402–17. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00748.x. PMID 15369522.
- Spiegel, D. R.; Lim, K. J. (2011). "A Case of Probable Korsakoff's Syndrome: A Syndrome of Frontal Lobe and Diencephalic Structural Pathogenesis and a Comparison with Medial Temporal Lobe Dementias". Innovations in Clinical Neuroscience. 8 (6): 15–19. PMC 3140893. PMID 21779537.
- Dayus, B.; Van Den Broek, M.D. (2000). "Treatment of stable delusional confabulations using self-monitoring training". Neuropsychological Rehabilitation. 10 (4): 415–27. doi:10.1080/096020100411998. S2CID 145657555.
- Johnson, M; Raye, C. L. (1998). "False memories and confabulation". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2 (4): 137–45. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(98)01152-8. PMID 21227110. S2CID 10033853.
- Langdon, R.; Turner, M (2010), "Delusion and confabulation: Overlapping or distinct distortions in reality?", Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 15 (1): 1–13, doi:10.1080/13546800903519095, PMID 20043251, S2CID 35177831
Further reading
- (2018), The Confabulating Mind. How the brain creates reality. 2nd edition, Oxford, New York: Oxford University Press, ISBN 9780198789680
- Hirstein, William (2005), Brain Fiction: Self-deception and the riddle of confabulation, Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press, ISBN 978-0-262-08338-6, retrieved 21 March 2012
- Sacks, Oliver (1985), "The Man Who Mistook His Wife for a Hat", The British Journal of Psychiatry: The Journal of Mental Science, 166 (1), New York: Perennial Library: 130–1, doi:10.1192/bjp.166.1.130, ISBN 978-0-06-097079-6, PMID 7894870
External links
Confabulation is a memory error consisting of the production of fabricated distorted or misinterpreted memories about oneself or the world It is generally associated with certain types of brain damage especially aneurysm in the anterior communicating artery or a specific subset of dementias While still an area of ongoing research the basal forebrain is implicated in the phenomenon of confabulation People who confabulate present with incorrect memories ranging from subtle inaccuracies to surreal fabrications and may include confusion or distortion in the temporal framing timing sequence or duration of memories In general they are very confident about their recollections even when challenged with contradictory evidence Confabulation occurs when individuals mistakenly recall false information without intending to deceive Brain damage dementia and anticholinergic toxidrome can cause this distortion Two types of confabulation exist provoked and spontaneous with two distinctions verbal and behavioral Verbal statements false information and the patient s unawareness of the distortion are all associated with this phenomenon Personality structure also plays a role in confabulation Numerous theories have been developed to explain confabulation Neuro psycho log i cal theories suggest that cognitive dysfunction causes the distortion Self identity theories posit that people confabulate to preserve themselves The temporality theory believes that confabulation occurs when an individual cannot place events properly in time The monitoring and strategic retrieval account theories argue that confabulation arises when individuals cannot recall memories correctly or monitor them after retrieval The executive control and fuzzy trace theories also attempt to explain why confabulation happens Confabulation can occur with nervous system injuries or illnesses including Korsakoff s syndrome Alzheimer s disease schizophrenia and traumatic brain injury It is believed that the right frontal lobe of the brain is damaged causing false memories Children are especially susceptible to forced confabulation as they are highly impressionable Feedback can increase confidence in false memories In rare cases confabulation occurs in ordinary individuals Different memory tests including recognition tasks and free recall tasks can be used to study confabulation Treatment depends on the underlying cause of the distortion Ongoing research aims to develop a standard test battery to discern between different types of confabulations distinguish delusions from confabulations understand the role of unconscious processes and identify pathological and nonpathological confabulations DescriptionConfabulation was originally defined as the emergence of memories of events and experiences which never took place Confabulation is distinguished from lying as there is no intent to deceive and the person is unaware the information is false Although individuals can present blatantly false information confabulation can also seem to be coherent internally consistent and relatively normal Most known cases of confabulation are symptomatic of brain damage or dementias such as aneurysm Alzheimer s disease or Wernicke Korsakoff syndrome a common manifestation of thiamine deficiency caused by alcohol use disorder Additionally confabulation often occurs in people with anticholinergic toxidrome when interrogated about bizarre or irrational behaviour Confabulated memories of all types most often occur in autobiographical memory and are indicative of a complicated and intricate process that can be led astray at any point during encoding storage or recall of a memory This type of confabulation is commonly seen in Korsakoff s syndrome Distinctions Several forms of confabulation have been distinguished which vary in terms of behaviour mechanism and location of brain damage The most common distinction is between the following two types Provoked confabulations represent a normal response to a faulty memory are common in both amnesia and dementia and can become apparent during memory tests Spontaneous confabulations do not occur in response to a cue and seem to be involuntary They are relatively rare more common in cases of dementia and may result from the interaction between frontal lobe pathology and organic amnesia A subgroup of patients at least occasionally act according to their confabulations betraying a confusion of current reality These patients are always disoriented about their current role location and time This form has been called behaviorally spontaneous confabulation It is strongly associated with damage disconnection or dysfunction of the posterior medial orbitofrontal cortex area 13 and appears to have a specific mechanism a failure of orbitofrontal reality filtering a preconscious mechanism normally verifying whether an upcoming thought refers to current reality or not Signs and symptomsConfabulation is associated with several characteristics False verbal statements that may include autobiographical and non personal information such as historical facts fairy tales or other aspects of semantic memory The account is normally coherent and is usually drawn from the patient s memory of actual experiences Very rarely it is fantastic Both the premise and the details of the account can be false The patient is unaware of the accounts distortions or inappropriateness and is not concerned when errors are pointed out There is no hidden motivation behind the account The patient s personality structure may play a role in their readiness to confabulate TheoriesTheories of confabulation range in emphasis Some theories propose that confabulations represent a way for memory disabled people to maintain their self identity Other theories use neurocognitive links to explain the process of confabulation Still other theories frame confabulation around the more familiar concept of delusion Other researchers frame confabulation within the fuzzy trace theory Finally some researchers call for theories that rely less on neurocognitive explanations and more on epistemic accounts Neuropsychological theories The most popular theories of confabulation come from the field of neuropsychology or cognitive neuroscience Controlled experimental evidence is however scarce Research suggests that confabulation is associated with dysfunction of cognitive processes that control the retrieval from long term memory Frontal lobe damage often disrupts this process preventing the retrieval of information and the evaluation of its output Furthermore researchers argue that confabulation is a disorder resulting from failed reality monitoring source monitoring i e deciding whether a memory is based on an actual event or whether it is imagined Some neuropsychologists suggest that errors in retrieval of information from long term memory that are made by normal subjects involve different components of control processes than errors made by confabulators Fantastic confabulations have been attributed to a dysfunction of the Supervisory System which is believed to be a function of the frontal cortex Temporality theory Support for the temporality account suggests that confabulations occur when an individual is unable to place events properly in time Thus an individual might correctly state an action they performed but say they did it yesterday when they did it weeks ago In the Memory Consciousness and Temporality Theory confabulation occurs because of a deficit in temporal consciousness or awareness Monitoring theory Along a similar notion are the theories of reality and source monitoring theories In these theories confabulation occurs when individuals incorrectly attribute memories as reality or incorrectly attribute memories to a certain source Thus an individual might claim an imagined event happened in reality or that a friend told him her about an event he she actually heard about on television Strategic retrieval account theory Supporters of the strategic retrieval account suggest that confabulations occur when an individual cannot actively monitor a memory for truthfulness after its retrieval An individual recalls a memory but there is some deficit after recall that interferes with the person establishing its falseness Executive control theory Still others propose that all types of false memories including confabulation fit into a general memory and executive function model In 2007 a framework for confabulation was proposed that stated confabulation is the result of two things Problems with executive control and problems with evaluation In the executive control deficit the incorrect memory is retrieved from the brain In the evaluative deficit the memory will be accepted as a truth due to an inability to distinguish a belief from an actual memory In the context of delusion theories Recent models of confabulation have attempted to build upon the link between delusion and confabulation More recently a monitoring account for delusion applied to confabulation proposed both the inclusion of conscious and unconscious processing The claim was that by encompassing the notion of both processes spontaneous versus provoked confabulations could be better explained In other words there are two ways to confabulate One is the unconscious spontaneous way in which a memory goes through no logical explanatory processing The other is the conscious provoked way in which a memory is recalled intentionally by the individual to explain something confusing or unusual Fuzzy trace theory Fuzzy trace theory or FTT is a concept more commonly applied to the explanation of judgement decisions According to this theory memories are encoded generally gist as well as specifically verbatim Thus a confabulation could result from recalling the incorrect verbatim memory or from being able to recall the gist portion but not the verbatim portion of a memory FTT uses a set of five principles to explain false memory phenomena Principle 1 suggests that subjects store verbatim information and gist information parallel to one another Both forms of storage involve the surface content of an experience Principle 2 shares factors of retrieval of gist and verbatim traces Principle 3 is based on dual opponent processes in false memory Generally gist retrieval supports false memory while verbatim retrieval suppresses it Developmental variability is the topic of Principle 4 As a child develops into an adult there is obvious improvement in the acquisition retention and retrieval of both verbatim and gist memory However during late adulthood there will be a decline in these abilities Finally Principle 5 explains that verbatim and gist processing cause vivid remembering Fuzzy trace Theory governed by these 5 principles has proved useful in explaining false memory and generating new predictions about it Epistemic theory However not all accounts are so embedded in the neurocognitive aspects of confabulation Some attribute confabulation to epistemic accounts In 2009 theories underlying the causation and mechanisms for confabulation were criticized for their focus on neural processes which are somewhat unclear as well as their emphasis on the negativity of false remembering Researchers proposed that an epistemic account of confabulation would be more encompassing of both the advantages and disadvantages of the process PresentationAssociated neurological and psychological conditions Confabulations are often symptoms of various syndromes and psychopathologies in the adult population including Korsakoff s syndrome Alzheimer s disease schizophrenia and traumatic brain injury Wernicke Korsakoff syndrome is a neurological disorder typically characterized by years of alcohol use disorder characterized by excessive alcohol consumption and a nutritional thiamine deficiency Confabulation is one salient symptom of this syndrome A study on confabulation in Korsakoff s patients found that they are subject to provoked confabulation when prompted with questions pertaining to episodic memory not semantic memory and when prompted with questions where the appropriate response would be I don t know This suggests that con fab u l ation in these patients is domain specific Korsakoff s patients who confabulate are more likely than healthy adults to falsely recognize distractor words suggesting that false recognition is a confabulatory behavior Alzheimer s disease is a condition with both neurological and psychological components It is a form of dementia associated with severe frontal lobe dys func tion Confabulation in individuals with Alzheimer s is often more spontaneous than it is in other conditions especially in the advanced stages of the disease Alzheimer s patients demonstrate comparable abilities to encode information as healthy elderly adults suggesting that impairments in encoding are not associated with confabulation However as seen in Korsakoff s patients confabulation in Alzheimer s patients is higher when prompted with questions investigating episodic memory Researchers suggest this is due to damage in the posterior cortical regions of the brain which is a symptom characteristic of Alzheimer s disease Schizophrenia is a psychological disorder in which confabulation is sometimes observed Although confabulation is usually coherent in its presentation con fab u l ations of schizophrenic patients are often delusional Researchers have noted that these patients tend to make up delusions on the spot which are often fantastic and become increasingly elaborate with questioning Unlike patients with Korsakoff s and Alzheimer s patients with schizophrenia are more likely to confabulate when prompted with questions regarding their semantic memories as opposed to episodic memory prompting In addition confabulation does not appear to be related to any memory deficit in schiz o phrenic patients This is contrary to most forms of confabulation Also confabulations made by schizophrenic patients often do not involve the creation of new information but instead involve an attempt by the patient to reconstruct actual details of a past event Traumatic brain injury TBI can also result in confabulation Research has shown that patients with damage to the inferior medial frontal lobe confabulate significantly more than patients with damage to the posterior area and healthy controls This suggests that this region is key in producing confabulatory responses and that memory deficit is important but not necessary in con fab u l ation Additionally research suggests that confabulation can be seen in patients with frontal lobe syndrome which involves an insult to the frontal lobe as a result of disease or traumatic brain injury TBI Finally rupture of the anterior or posterior communicating artery subarachnoid hemorrhage and encephalitis are also possible causes of confabulation Location of brain lesions Confabulation is believed to be a result of damage to the right frontal lobe of the brain In particular damage can be localized to the ventromedial frontal lobes and other structures fed by the anterior communicating artery ACoA including the basal forebrain septum fornix cingulate gyrus cingulum anterior hypothalamus and head of the caudate nucleus Behaviourally spontaneous confabulation may occur in the context of dementia or the Wernicke Korsakoff syndrome where brain damage is difficult to localise If it is due to circumscribed brain damage e g after ruptured aneurysm of the anterior communicating artery traumatic brain injury stroke lesions involved the posterior medial orbitofrontal cortex area 13 or an area directly connected with it Developmental differences While some recent literature has suggested that older adults may be more susceptible than their younger counterparts to have false memories the majority of research on forced confabulation centers around children Children are particularly susceptible to forced confabulations based on their high suggestibility When forced to recall confabulated events children are less likely to remember that they had previously confabulated these situations and they are more likely than their adult counterparts to come to remember these confabulations as real events that transpired Research suggests that this inability to distinguish between past confabulatory and real events is centered on developmental differences in source monitoring Due to underdeveloped encoding and critical reasoning skills children s ability to distinguish real memories from false memories may be impaired It may also be that younger children lack the meta memory processes required to remember confabulated versus non confabulated events Children s meta memory processes may also be influenced by expectancies or biases in that they believe that highly plausible false scenarios are not confabulated However when knowingly being tested for accuracy children are more likely to respond I don t know at a rate comparable to adults for unanswerable questions than they are to confabulate Ultimately misinformation effects can be minimized by tailoring individual interviews to the specific developmental stage often based on age of the participant Provoked versus spontaneous confabulations There is evidence to support different cognitive mechanisms for provoked and spontaneous confabulation One study suggested that spontaneous confabulation may be a result of an amnesic patient s inability to distinguish the chronological order of events in their memory In contrast provoked confabulation may be a compensatory mechanism in which the patient tries to make up for their memory deficiency by attempting to demonstrate competency in recollection Confidence in false memories Confabulation of events or situations may lead to an eventual acceptance of the confabulated information as true For instance people who knowingly lie about a situation may eventually come to believe that their lies are truthful with time In an interview setting people are more likely to confabulate in situations in which they are presented false information by another person as opposed to when they self generate these falsehoods Further people are more likely to accept false information as true when they are interviewed at a later time after the event in question than those who are interviewed immediately or soon after the event Affirmative feedback for confabulated responses is also shown to increase the confabulator s confidence in their response For instance in culprit identification if a witness falsely identifies a member of a line up he will be more confident in his identification if the interviewer provides affirmative feedback This effect of confirmatory feedback appears to last over time as witnesses will even remember the confabulated information months later Among normal subjects On rare occasions confabulation can also be seen in normal subjects It is currently unclear how completely healthy individuals produce confabulations It is possible that these individuals are in the process of developing some type of organic condition that is causing their confabulation symptoms It is not uncommon however for the general population to display some very mild symptoms of provoked confabulations Subtle distortions and intrusions in memory are commonly produced by normal subjects when they remember something poorly Diagnosis and treatmentSpontaneous confabulations due to their involuntary nature cannot be manipulated in a laboratory setting However provoked confabulations can be researched in various theoretical contexts The mechanisms found to underlie provoked confabulations can be applied to spontaneous confabulation mechanisms The basic premise of researching confabulation comprises finding errors and distortions in memory tests of an individual Deese Roediger McDermott lists Confabulations can be detected in the context of the Deese Roediger McDermott paradigm by using the Deese Roediger McDermott lists Participants listen to audio recordings of several lists of words centered around a theme known as the critical word The participants are later asked to recall the words on their list If the participant recalls the critical word which was never explicitly stated in the list it is considered a confabulation Participants often have a false memory for the critical word Recognition tasks Confabulations can also be researched by using continuous recognition tasks These tasks are often used in conjunction with confidence ratings Generally in a recognition task participants are rapidly presented with pictures Some of these pictures are shown once others are shown multiple times Participants press a key if they have seen the picture previously Following a period of time participants repeat the task More errors on the second task versus the first are indicative of confusion representing false memories Free recall tasks Confabulations can also be detected using a free recall task such as a self narrative task Participants are asked to recall stories semantic or autobiographical that are highly familiar to them The stories recalled are encoded for errors that could be classified as distortions in memory Distortions could include falsifying true story elements or including details from a completely different story Errors such as these would be indicative of confabulations Treatment Treatment for confabulation is somewhat dependent on the cause or source if identifiable For example treatment of Wernicke Korsakoff syndrome involves large doses of vitamin B in order to reverse the thiamine deficiency If there is no known physiological cause more general cognitive techniques may be used to treat confabulation A case study published in 2000 showed that SMT reduced delusional confabulations Furthermore improvements were maintained at a three month follow up and were found to generalize to everyday settings Although this treatment seems promising more rigorous research is necessary to determine the efficacy of SMT in the general confabulation population ResearchAlthough significant gains have been made in the understanding of confabulation in recent years there is still much to be learned One group of researchers in particular has laid out several important questions for future study They suggest more information is needed regarding the neural systems that support the different cognitive processes necessary for normal source monitoring They also proposed the idea of developing a standard neuropsychological test battery able to discriminate between the different types of confabulations And there is a considerable amount of debate regarding the best approach to organizing and combining neuro imaging pharmacological and cognitive behavioral approaches to understand confabulation In a recent review article another group of researchers contemplate issues concerning the distinctions between delusions and confabulation They question whether delusions and confabulation should be considered distinct or overlapping disorders and if overlapping to what degree They also discuss the role of unconscious processes in confabulation Some researchers suggest that unconscious emotional and motivational processes are potentially just as important as cognitive and memory problems Finally they raise the question of where to draw the line between the pathological and the nonpathological Delusion like beliefs and confabulation like fabrications are commonly seen in healthy individuals What are the important differences between patients with similar etiology who do and do not confabulate Since the line between pathological and nonpathological is likely blurry should we take a more dimensional approach to confabulation Research suggests that confabulation occurs along a continuum of implausibility bizarreness content conviction preoccupation and distress and impact on daily life See alsoCompare with Anosognosia Confabulation neural networks Cryptomnesia False memory Hallucination Hallucination artificial intelligence Hindsight bias Misinformation effect Revelation Rosy retrospection Not to be confused with Scams swindles Confidence tricks Fraud Hoaxes Gaslighting PhishingReferencesBerrios G E 1998 Confabulations A Conceptual History Journal of the History of the Neurosciences 7 225 241 Pendick Daniel Summer 2000 The truth about confabulation Memory Loss amp the Brain Newsletter of the Memory Disorders Project Rutgers University Newark Archived from the original on 3 January 2013 Retrieved 7 April 2022 Matthews Paul M McClelland James L 2010 Nalbantian Suzanne ed The memory process neuroscientific and humanistic perspectives Cambridge Massachusetts MIT Press ISBN 978 0 262 01457 1 Wernicke Carl 1900 Grundriss der Psychiatrie in klinischen Vorlesungen Leipzig Thieme p 139 Schnider Armin 2018 The Confabulating Mind How the brain creates reality 2nd edition Oxford New York Oxford University Press ISBN 9780198789680 Moscovitch M 1995 Confabulation In Eds Schacter D L Coyle J T Fischbach G D Mesulum M M amp Sullivan L G Memory Distortion pp 226 51 Cambridge Massachusetts Harvard University Press Sivolap IuP Damulin IV 2013 Wernicke encephalopathy and Korsakoff s psychosis clinical pathophysiological correlation diagnostics and treatment Zh Nevrol Psikhiatr Im S S Korsakova 113 6 Pt 2 20 26 PMID 23887463 Kopelman M D Thomson A D Guerrini I Marshall E J 2009 The Korsakoff Syndrome Clinical Aspects Psychology and Treatment Alcohol and Alcoholism 44 2 148 54 doi 10 1093 alcalc agn118 PMID 19151162 Nahum L Bouzerda Wahlen A Guggisberg A Ptak R Schnider A 2012 Forms of confabulation dissociations and associations Neuropsychologia 50 10 2524 34 doi 10 1016 j neuropsychologia 2012 06 026 PMID 22781813 Kopelman M D 1987 Two types of confabulation Journal of Neurology Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 50 11 1482 87 doi 10 1136 jnnp 50 11 1482 PMC 1032561 PMID 3694207 Metcalf Kasey Langdon Robyn Coltheart Max 1 February 2007 Models of confabulation A critical review and a new framework Cognitive Neuropsychology 24 1 23 47 doi 10 1080 02643290600694901 PMID 18416482 S2CID 45885109 Gilboa A 13 April 2006 Mechanisms of spontaneous confabulations a strategic retrieval account Brain 129 6 1399 1414 doi 10 1093 brain awl093 PMID 16638795 Schnider A von Daniken C Gutbrod K 1996 The mechanisms of spontaneous and provoked confabulations Brain A Journal of Neurology 119 Pt 4 4 1365 75 doi 10 1093 brain 119 4 1365 PMID 8813298 Schnider A Ptak R 1999 Spontaneous confabulators fail to suppress currently irrelevant memory traces Nature Neuroscience 2 7 677 81 doi 10 1038 10236 PMID 10404203 Schnider A 2003 Spontaneous confabulation and the adaptation of thought to ongoing reality Nature Reviews Neuroscience 4 8 662 71 doi 10 1038 nrn1179 PMID 12894241 Schnider A 2013 Orbitofrontal reality filtering Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 7 67 doi 10 3389 fnbeh 2013 00067 PMC 3677127 PMID 23772208 Glowinski Remy Payman Vahid Frencham Kate 2008 Confabulation a spontaneous and fantastic review Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 42 11 932 40 doi 10 1080 00048670802415335 PMID 18941957 S2CID 23586700 Kopelman Michael D 2010 Varieties of confabulation and delusion Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 15 1 3 14 37 doi 10 1080 13546800902732830 PMID 19753493 S2CID 19764950 Brainerd C J Reyna V F 1 November 1998 Fuzzy Trace Theory and Children s False Memories Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 71 2 81 129 doi 10 1006 jecp 1998 2464 PMID 9843617 S2CID 12290995 Bortolotti Lisa Cox Rochelle E 1 December 2009 Faultless ignorance Strengths and limitations of epistemic definitions of confabulation Consciousness and Cognition 18 4 952 65 doi 10 1016 j concog 2009 08 011 PMID 19773185 S2CID 35121072 Baddeley Alan Wilson Barbara 1986 Amnesia autobiographical memory and confabulation In Rubin David C ed Autobiographical Memory Cambridge Cambridge University Press pp 225 52 doi 10 1017 CBO9780511558313 020 ISBN 9780511558313 Moscovitch M Melo B 1997 Strategic retrieval and the frontal lobes evidence from confabulation and amnesia Neuropsychologia 35 7 1017 34 doi 10 1016 s0028 3932 97 00028 6 PMID 9226662 S2CID 33862514 Johnson M K 1991 Reality monitoring Evidence from confabulation in organic brain disease patients In G P Prigatano amp D L Schacter Eds Awareness of deficit after brain injury pp 176 97 New York Oxford Burgess P W Shallice T 1996 Confabulation and the control of recollection Memory 4 4 359 411 doi 10 1080 096582196388906 PMID 8817460 Norman D A amp Shallice T 1980 Attention to action Willed and automatic control of behavior University of California San Diego CHIP Report 99 Later published as Norman Donald A Shallice Tim 1986 Attention to Action Willed and Automatic Control of Behavior Consciousness and Self Regulation pp 1 18 doi 10 1007 978 1 4757 0629 1 1 ISBN 978 1 4757 0631 4 Dalla Barba Gianfranco Boisse Marie Francoise 2010 Temporal consciousness and confabulation Is the medial temporal lobe temporal Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 15 1 3 95 117 doi 10 1080 13546800902758017 PMID 19750399 S2CID 42447646 Kopelman Michael D 1 May 1999 Varieties of False Memory Cognitive Neuropsychology 16 3 5 197 214 doi 10 1080 026432999380762 Turner Martha Coltheart Max 2010 Confabulation and delusion A common monitoring framework Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 15 1 3 346 76 doi 10 1080 13546800903441902 PMID 20043250 S2CID 205768903 Brainerd C J amp Reyna V F 2002 Fuzzy Trace Theory and False Memory Current Directions In Psychological Science Wiley Blackwell 11 5 164 69 Homewood J Bond N W 1999 Thiamin deficiency and Korsakoff s syndrome Failure to find memory impairments following nonalcoholic Wernicke s encephalopathy Alcohol 19 1 75 84 doi 10 1016 s0741 8329 99 00027 0 PMID 10487391 Dalla Barba G Cipolotti L Denes G 1990 Autobiographical memory loss and confabulation in Korsakoff s syndrome A case report Cortex 26 4 525 34 doi 10 1016 s0010 9452 13 80302 4 PMID 2081390 S2CID 4481075 Kessels RP Kortrijk HE Wester AJ Nys GM Confabulation behavior and false memories in Korsakoff s syndrome role of source memory and executive functioning Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2008 Apr 62 2 220 25 Damme Ilse d Ydewalle Gery 2010 Confabulation versus experimentally induced false memories in Korsakoff patients Journal of Neuropsychology 4 2 211 30 doi 10 1348 174866409X478231 PMID 19930792 Cooper Janine M Shanks Michael F Venneri Annalena 11 May 2006 Provoked confabulations in Alzheimer s disease Neuropsychologia 44 10 1697 1707 doi 10 1016 j neuropsychologia 2006 03 029 PMID 16697019 S2CID 41292107 Wing J K Cooper J E Sartorius N 1974 The description and classification of psychiatric symptoms An instruction manual for the PSE and catego system London Cambridge University Press Lorente Rovira E Pomarol Clotet E McCarthy R A Berrios G E McKenna P J 2007 Confabulation in schizophrenia and its relationship to clinical and neuropsychological features of the disorder Psychological Medicine 37 10 1403 12 doi 10 1017 S0033291707000566 PMID 17506924 S2CID 32121170 Lorente Rovira E Santos Gomez J L Moro M Villagran J M McKenna P J 1 November 2010 Confabulation in schizophrenia A neuropsychological study Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 16 6 1018 26 doi 10 1017 S1355617710000718 hdl 10234 88477 PMID 20630120 S2CID 23194952 Turner Martha S Cipolotti Lisa Yousry Tarek A Shallice Tim 1 June 2008 Confabulation Damage to a specific inferior medial prefrontal system PDF Cortex 44 6 637 48 doi 10 1016 j cortex 2007 01 002 PMID 18472034 S2CID 17414598 Baddeley A Wilson B 1988 Frontal amnesia and the dysexecutive syndrome Brain and Cognition 7 2 212 30 doi 10 1016 0278 2626 88 90031 0 PMID 3377900 S2CID 26954876 Papagno C Baddeley A 1997 Confabulation in a dysexecutive patient Implications for models of retrieval Cortex 33 4 743 52 doi 10 1016 s0010 9452 08 70731 7 PMID 9444475 S2CID 4484799 Baddeley Alan 1996 Exploring the Central Executive Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 49A 1 5 28 doi 10 1080 713755608 S2CID 54633639 Dalla Barba G 1993 Confabulation knowledge and recollective experience Cognitive Neuropsychology 10 1 1 20 doi 10 1080 02643299308253454 Alexander M P Freedman M 1984 Amnesia after anterior communicating artery aneurysm rupture Neurology 34 6 752 57 doi 10 1212 wnl 34 6 752 PMID 6539437 S2CID 34357231 Irle E Wowra B Kunert H J Hampl J Kunze S 1992 Memory disturbances following anterior communicating artery rupture Annals of Neurology 31 5 473 80 doi 10 1002 ana 410310503 PMID 1596082 S2CID 42233017 Brainerd C J Reyna V F Ceci S J 2008 Developmental reversals in false memory A review of data and theory Psychological Bulletin 134 3 343 82 doi 10 1037 0033 2909 134 3 343 PMID 18444700 Shapiro Lauren R Purdy Telisa L 2005 Suggestibility and source monitoring errors blame the interview style interviewer consistency and the child s personality Applied Cognitive Psychology 19 4 489 506 doi 10 1002 acp 1093 Shapiro Lauren R Blackford Cheryl Chen Chiung Fen 2005 Eyewitness memory for a simulated misdemeanor crime the role of age and temperament in suggestibility Applied Cognitive Psychology 19 3 267 89 doi 10 1002 acp 1089 Ackil Jennifer K Zaragoza Maria S 1 November 1998 Memorial consequences of forced confabulation Age differences in susceptibility to false memories Developmental Psychology 34 6 1358 72 doi 10 1037 0012 1649 34 6 1358 PMID 9823517 Ghetti Simona Castelli Paola Lyons Kristen E 2010 Knowing about not remembering developmental dissociations in lack of memory monitoring Developmental Science 13 4 611 21 doi 10 1111 j 1467 7687 2009 00908 x PMID 20590725 Ghetti Simona Alexander Kristen Weede 2004 If It Happened I Would Remember It Strategic Use of Event Memorability in the Rejection of False Autobiographical Events Child Development 75 2 542 61 doi 10 1111 j 1467 8624 2004 00692 x PMID 15056205 Roebers Claudia Fernandez Olivia 2002 The Effects of Accuracy Motivation on Children s and Adults Event Recall Suggestibility and Their Answers to Unanswerable Questions Journal of Cognition and Development 3 4 415 43 doi 10 1207 S15327647JCD3 4 03 Scoboria Alan Mazzoni Giuliana Kirsch Irving 2008 Don t know responding to answerable and unanswerable questions during misleading and hypnotic interviews Journal of Experimental Psychology Applied 14 3 255 65 doi 10 1037 1076 898X 14 3 255 PMID 18808279 Holliday Robyn E Albon Amanda J 2004 Minimising misinformation effects in young children with cognitive interview mnemonics Applied Cognitive Psychology 18 3 263 81 doi 10 1002 acp 973 Schnider Armin von Daniken Christine Gutbrod Klemens 19 February 1996 The mechanisms of spontaneous and provoked confabulations Brain 119 4 1365 75 doi 10 1093 brain 119 4 1365 PMID 8813298 Pickel Kerri 2004 When a lie becomes the truth The effects of self generated misinformation on eyewitness memory Memory 12 1 14 26 doi 10 1080 09658210244000072 PMID 15098618 S2CID 21383980 Polage Danielle C 2004 Fabrication deflation The mixed effects of lying on memory Applied Cognitive Psychology 18 4 455 65 doi 10 1002 acp 995 Pezdek Kathy Lam Shirley T Sperry Kathryn 2009 Forced confabulation more strongly influences event memory if suggestions are other generated than self generated Legal and Criminological Psychology 14 2 241 52 doi 10 1348 135532508X344773 Melnyk Laura Bruck Maggie 2004 Timing moderates the effects of repeated suggestive interviewing on children s eyewitness memory Applied Cognitive Psychology 18 5 613 31 doi 10 1002 acp 1013 Hafstad Gertrud Sofie Memon Amina Logie Robert 2004 Post identification feedback confidence and recollections of witnessing conditions in child witnesses Applied Cognitive Psychology 18 7 901 12 doi 10 1002 acp 1037 Zaragoza M S Payment K E Ackil J K Drivdahl S B Beck M 2001 Interviewing Witnesses Forced Confabulation and Confirmatory Feedback Increase False Memories Psychological Science 12 6 473 77 doi 10 1111 1467 9280 00388 PMID 11760134 S2CID 11951759 Howe Mark L Cicchetti Dante Toth Sheree L Cerrito Beth M 1 September 2004 True and False Memories in Maltreated Children Child Development 75 5 1402 17 doi 10 1111 j 1467 8624 2004 00748 x PMID 15369522 Spiegel D R Lim K J 2011 A Case of Probable Korsakoff s Syndrome A Syndrome of Frontal Lobe and Diencephalic Structural Pathogenesis and a Comparison with Medial Temporal Lobe Dementias Innovations in Clinical Neuroscience 8 6 15 19 PMC 3140893 PMID 21779537 Dayus B Van Den Broek M D 2000 Treatment of stable delusional confabulations using self monitoring training Neuropsychological Rehabilitation 10 4 415 27 doi 10 1080 096020100411998 S2CID 145657555 Johnson M Raye C L 1998 False memories and confabulation Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2 4 137 45 doi 10 1016 S1364 6613 98 01152 8 PMID 21227110 S2CID 10033853 Langdon R Turner M 2010 Delusion and confabulation Overlapping or distinct distortions in reality Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 15 1 1 13 doi 10 1080 13546800903519095 PMID 20043251 S2CID 35177831Further reading 2018 The Confabulating Mind How the brain creates reality 2nd edition Oxford New York Oxford University Press ISBN 9780198789680 Hirstein William 2005 Brain Fiction Self deception and the riddle of confabulation Cambridge Massachusetts MIT Press ISBN 978 0 262 08338 6 retrieved 21 March 2012 Sacks Oliver 1985 The Man Who Mistook His Wife for a Hat The British Journal of Psychiatry The Journal of Mental Science 166 1 New York Perennial Library 130 1 doi 10 1192 bjp 166 1 130 ISBN 978 0 06 097079 6 PMID 7894870External links